Content » Vol 48, Issue 2

Original report

Rasch analysis of measurement instruments capturing psychological personal factors in persons with spinal cord injury

Claudio Peter, PhD1,2, Stefan E. Schulenberg, PhD3,4, Erin M. Buchanan, PhD5, Birgit Prodinger, PhD1,2 and Szilvia Geyh, PhD1,2, for the SwiSCI Study Group

From the 1Swiss Paraplegic Research (SPF), Nottwil, 2Department of Health Sciences and Health Policy, University of Lucerne, Lucerne, Switzerland, 3Department of Psychology, University of Mississippi, 4University of Mississippi Clinical-Disaster Research Center (UM-CDRC), Oxford and 5Department of Psychology, Missouri State University, Springfield, USA

OBJECTIVE: To evaluate the metric properties of distinct measures of psychological personal factors comprising feelings, beliefs, motives, and patterns of experience and behaviour assessed in the Swiss Spinal Cord Injury Cohort Study (SwiSCI), using Rasch methodology.

METHODS: SwiSCI Pathway 2 is a community-based, nationwide, cross-sectional survey for persons with spinal cord injury (SCI) (n = 511). The Rasch partial credit model was used for each subscale of the Positive Affect Negative Affect Scale (PANAS), Appraisal of Life Events Scale (ALE), Purpose in Life test – Short Form (PIL-SF), and the Big Five Inventory-K (BFI-K).

RESULTS: The measures were unidimensional, with the exception of the positive affect items of the PANAS, where pairwise t-tests resulted in 10% significant cases, indicating multidimensionality. The BFI-K subscale agreeableness revealed low reliability (0.53). Other reliability estimates ranged between 0.61 and 0.89. Ceiling and floor effects were found for most measures. SCI-related differential item functioning (DIF) was rarely found. Language DIF was identified for several items of the BFI-K, PANAS and the ALE, but not for the PIL-SF.

CONCLUSION: A majority of the measures satisfy the assumptions of the Rasch model, including unidimensionality. Invariance across language versions still represents a major challenge.

Key words: psychological factors; personality; affect; spinal cord injury; validity; reliability; psychometrics.

J Rehabil Med 2016; 48: 175–188

Correspondence address: Claudio Peter, Participation & Social Integration Unit, Swiss Paraplegic Research, CH-6207 Nottwil, Switzerland. E-mail: claudio.peter@paraplegie.ch

Accepted Sep 21, 2015; Epub ahead of print Feb 1, 2016

INTRODUCTION

The impact of a spinal cord injury (SCI) on an individual is severe, affecting functioning across a range of domains (1). A full understanding of the lived experience of SCI includes capturing the psychological perspective of an individual. SCI can influence an individual’s feelings, thoughts and beliefs, and life goals. Indeed, diverse well-being and mental health trajectories have been observed. Some individuals show positive adjustment patterns, while others experience poor adjustment (2).

Psychological personal factors (PPF), however, can also act as resources, and hence, determinants of adjustment following SCI (3). Literature overviews have identified associations between higher well-being and higher self-efficacy, purpose in life, and other PPF (4, 5). The increased interest in PPF is also grounded in their malleability: state-like factors represent potential intervention targets. For example, several multi-modal intervention programmes have resulted in increased self-efficacy levels in persons with SCI (6–8).

To capture the lived experience of SCI, several PPF were included and assessed in the Swiss Spinal Cord Injury Cohort Study (SwiSCI) pathway 2 (9). The survey comprised different areas of PPF, including feelings, thoughts and beliefs, motives, and patterns of experience and behaviour (Table I).

Table I. Areas of psychological personal factors included in the Swiss Spinal Cord Injury Cohort Study (SwiSCI) Domain Set Pathway 2, the specifications, and measurement instruments, information on metric analysis

Areas of psychological personal factors

Specifications

Measurement

Metric analysis

Basic socio-demographic personal characteristics

Age, sex, nationality, citizenship, educational background, occupational background, economic background, religious affiliation

 

Not meaningful

Positive in the immediate social and physical context

Marital status/relationship status/children

Items on these characteristics

Not meaningful

For each of the following areas, one instrument will be used as a case in point (in bold)

Feelings

Positive and negative affect

Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS) (14)

Meaningful

Symptoms of depression

Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale (HADS) (50)

Already conducted (51)

Thoughts and beliefs

Appraisals

Appraisal of Life Events Scale (ALE) (15)

Meaningful

Life satisfaction

WHOQOL BREF – selected items (52, 53)

Already conducted (53)

Motives

Purpose in life

Purpose in Life test – Short Form

(PIL-SF) (16)

Meaningful

Patterns of experience and behaviour

Coping

Personality

Social skills

Self-efficacy

Brief COPE (54)

Big Five Inventory (BFI-K) (17, 28)

Social Skills Inventory (SSI) (55)

General Self-Efficacy Scale (56)

Meaningful

Meaningful

Meaningful

Already conducted (57)

 

Psychometric evaluations of measurement instruments are prerequisites for their valid use in research and clinical settings. Until now, classical test-theoretical approaches (CTT), such as confirmatory factor analyses, have been used for validation. However, these approaches have recently been supplemented by modern test-theoretical approaches, such as the unidimensional Rasch model (10), which adds to our understanding of aspects related, in particular, to internal construct validity not covered by CTT (11). One key criticism of CTT lies in its assumption of a continuous nature of a categorical response system without empirical evidence. In Rasch analyses, the observed response patterns are tested against the probabilistic response expectations of the Rasch model, and do not assume that the observed variables are of a continuous nature (12). Finding confirmation of data fit, the Rasch model ensures the additivity of the raw sum score of a measure (12, 13). In the context of a national, epidemiological survey, such as SwiSCI, reliability, adequate targeting and invariance of measures across different national languages are further key aspects that can be investigated using Rasch analyses. Hence, Rasch analyses provide the basis for a comprehensive evaluation of the quality of the measures and provide trend-setting indications regarding future use within SwiSCI and other population surveys.

The objective of the current study is to evaluate the metric properties of distinct measures of PPF comprising feelings, thoughts and beliefs, motives, and patterns of experience and behaviour that were assessed in SwiSCI pathway 2 using Rasch methodology. More specifically, we focus on the Positive Affect Negative Affect Scale (PANAS) (14), the Appraisal of Life Events Scale (ALE) (15), the Purpose in Life test – Short Form (PIL-SF) (16) and the Big Five Inventory-K (BFI-K) (17).

METHODS

Study design

Pathway 2 of SwiSCI is a community-based, nationwide, cross-sectional survey. The main aim of SwiSCI is to provide basic information to support the health, functioning, and quality of life of persons with SCI living in Switzerland. A detailed description of the study design of SwiSCI can be found elsewhere (18).

Participants

Individuals 16 years and older living in the Swiss community with a traumatic (e.g. due to accident) or non-traumatic (e.g. internal bleeding) SCI were eligible for SwiSCI. SCI is differentiated by the lesion level (paraplegia vs tetraplegia) and the completeness of a lesion (complete vs incomplete). Individuals with congenital conditions, such as spina bifida, new SCI in the context of palliative care, or neurodegenerative disorders, such as multiple sclerosis, were excluded from SwiSCI. Participants signed an informed consent form, and local ethics committees approved the study.

Procedure

The community-based survey consisted of self-report questionnaires, which were sent to eligible persons in 3 waves from September 2011 to March 2013. The first wave included general information about SwiSCI, the informed consent form, and questions regarding socio-demographic and lesion-related variables. Individuals who returned a signed consent form and the first questionnaire received a second questionnaire on health, functioning, and well-being. In the third wave, 3 randomized samples, stratified by sex, age, and level of lesion were drawn from the respondents of the second wave. These samples each received a different questionnaire module. The measures used in this study were assessed in the Psychological Personal Factors and Health Behavior Module (PPF-HB).

Measurement instruments

Feelings. The 20-item Positive Affect Negative Affect Scale (PANAS) (14, 19–21) was derived from a principal components analysis of the mood checklist (22) and consists of the 2 subscales positive affect (PA) and negative affect (NA). PA and NA reflect distinct dimensions, with high PA representing the extent to which an individual experiences pleasurable engagement with the environment and high NA characterizing subjective distress and engagement with negative experiences. Respondents were asked to rate the degree to which they have experienced each particular emotion within the past week, with reference to a 5-point scale (1 = ”very slightly or not at all” to 5 = ”very much”). Confirmatory factor analysis supported the construct validity of the PANAS subscales, and reliability estimates were very good (14).

Thoughts and beliefs

Appraisals. Primary appraisals, i.e. the cognitive evaluation of a specific stimulus, were measured with the Appraisal of Life Events Scale (ALE) (15). Using 16 adjectives and a 6-point scale, respondents appraised difficult life-events over the past 3 months. The adjectives refer to 3 dimensions: threat (e.g. “terrifying”, 6 items), challenge (e.g. “stimulating”, 6 items) and loss (e.g. “pitiful”, 4 items). Total scores range from 0 to 20 (loss), or 0 to 30 respectively (threat, challenge). Higher scores indicate higher appraisal levels, e.g. a higher extent of appraising the stimulus as challenge (or threat or loss, respectively). Good internal reliability and convergent validity have been reported (15, 23).

Motives

Purpose in life. Purpose in life (PIL) was assessed using the Purpose in Life test – Short Form (PIL-SF). It consists of 4 items with different anchors (16), which are all based on the original 20-item version (24, 25). The PIL-SF measures the extent to which persons recognize or achieve progress towards their life goals or generally perceive a sense of purpose in life. An example item is “I have discovered: no mission or purpose in life vs a satisfying life purpose.” The item response scale ranges from 1 to 7, leading to a total score of 4–28. Higher scores indicate higher PIL. Good internal consistency, unidimensionality, as well as divergent and convergent validity, have been reported (16, 26, 27).

Patterns of experience and behaviour

Personality. The 21-item Big Five Inventory-K (BFI-K) was used to assess the “Big Five” personality characteristics openness (5 items), conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness, and neuroticism (4 items each). The BFI-K uses a 5-point Likert-type response scale. It was adapted from the original 44-item BFI to provide a short economic measure of the 5 personality characteristics for contexts with limited time resources necessitating the use of brief measures (17, 28–30). Good reliability and validity data have been reported (17).

Rasch analysis

We applied the Rasch Partial Credit Model (31) for each subscale of the PANAS (positive affect, negative affect), the ALE (challenge, threat, loss), the BFI-K (openness, conscientiousness, extraversion, agreeableness, neuroticism) and the PIL-SF (purpose in life). For all subscales we tested: (i) stochastic ordering, (ii) unidimensionality and local independency, (iii) reliability, (iv) structure of the response scale, (v) targeting, and (vi) item bias or differential item functioning (DIF) using RUMM2030 software (32).

First, stochastic ordering (fit) was tested by comparing the observed data with the Rasch-model implied expectations for all items with a χ2 based goodness of fit test. The Rasch model posits that persons with a higher ability are more likely to answer an item correctly. With regard to PPF, this fit indicates that persons with, e.g. high PIL are expected to achieve higher levels on the PIL-SF compared with persons with lower PIL. Furthermore, standardized residuals (z values) and χ2 results for each item were calculated. Z values above ± 2.50 and significant χ2 tests indicate misfit to the Rasch model (33).

Secondly, items were grouped according to their loading with the first residual PCA factor, and pairwise t-tests were calculated for each person to assess unidimensionality. The number of significant t-tests, i.e. the lower bound of the 95% confidence interval (95% CI), should be below 5% to indicate unidimensionality (34). Multidimensionality can cause disordered thresholds or DIF (see below). Testlets, i.e. combining items to a superior latent construct, were created to examine for these possible effects and to investigate common variance between the testlets (35).

Local independency was evaluated to further investigate the assumptions of the Rasch model. Local independency means that the response to 1 item is not connected with the response to another item, conditional on the trait. Correlation between the items should be based solely on the measured trait; therefore, residual correlations should be close to zero (36). Positive residual correlations above 0.30 indicate local dependency (37). Local dependency can be tackled by the creation of testlets (35). Under a testlet solution the amount of common variance retained by the aggregation of single items into a super-item is reported. In RUMM2030 this is examined via a bi-factor model, whereby the common variance retained in the total score is indicated by the A statistic (38). A value of 0.88 indicates that the unidimensional latent estimate is based upon at least 88% of the total variance.

Next, reliability was assessed using the Person Separation Index (PSI). As with Cronbach’s alpha, a value higher than 0.80 indicates very good reliability for the purpose of group comparisons (39, 40).

Fourth, the structure of the response scale was tested by studying the ordering of the thresholds of each item. Thresholds are boundaries between the response scale options. For example, a 5-point scale has 4 thresholds. The thresholds need increasing values to be ordered, representing the move from low to high levels on the logit continuum of a construct. Disordered thresholds indicate a response scale that does not work as envisioned (41). Disordered thresholds can be adjusted by collapsing response options.

Fifth, targeting was investigated by checking the distribution of person and item threshold parameters across the continuum of the construct. To assess potential floor or ceiling effects, the percentage of individuals with the minimum and maximum raw total score was calculated (42). Furthermore, the difference between the mean person location on the logit continuum and the mean item location (by definition = 0.00) was checked, under consideration of the 95% CI.

Sixth, DIF was checked regarding age, sex, education, severity of injury, combined time and age since injury, as well as language and administration mode of the questionnaires. DIF, or item bias, reflects the notion that persons in different groups (e.g. paraplegia vs tetraplegia) respond differently to an item, even though they share the same trait level (12). To address potential DIF, the standardized residuals between the groups and across the person parameter continuum were analysed with a 2-way analysis of variance (ANOVA). DIF is indicated by a significant main effect of the group (uniform DIF; e.g. injury level), or a significant interaction effect (non-uniform DIF; e.g. injury level × PIL), using Bonferroni correction to reduce Type I error (43). In the case of DIF, we split the DIF item to test whether it caused item misfit or compensatory DIF (44). In the case of language DIF, we re-analysed DIF without the Italian sample to check whether item biases were stable, or due to the small Italian sample size. We also evaluated the impact of all DIF by investigating the differences in the mean locations between the groups for all class intervals in the DIF items. A difference of < 0.5 logit was considered as inconsequential, and item split was not retained in the final analysis (45, 46).

RESULTS

Overall, 511 persons completed the PPF-HB questionnaires, with 361 individuals using the German, 129 the French, and 21 the Italian language versions. Socio-demographic and lesion-related characteristics of the sample are presented in Table II. The findings of the Rasch analyses will be discussed for each scale (Tables III–VII).

Table II. Socio-demographic and lesion-related characteristics of the study participants (n = 511)

 

Participants

Age, years, mean (SD)

52.91 (14.79)

 

Missing

0

Sex, n

 

 

Male

371 (72.6)

 

Female

140 (27.4)

 

Missing

0

Education, years, mean (SD)

13.78 (3.34)

 

Missing

12 (2.3)

Age at SCI, years, mean (SD)

35.38 (17.60)

 

Missing

5 (1.0)

Severity of lesion, n

 

 

Complete paraplegia

166 (32.5)

 

Complete tetraplegia

56 (11.0)

 

Incomplete paraplegia

184 (36.0)

 

Incomplete tetraplegia

102 (20.0)

 

Missing

3 (0.6)

Cause of injury, n

 

 

Traumatic

400 (78.3)

 

Non-traumatic

109 (21.3)

 

Missing

2 (0.4)

Time since injury, months, mean (SD)

209.35 (156.73)

 

Missing

5 (1.0)

Mode of administration of questionnaire, n

 

 

Online

218 (42.7)

 

Paper-form

291 (56.9)

 

Interview

2 (0.4)

 

Missing

0

Language of questionnaire, n

 

 

German

361 (70.6)

 

French

129 (25.2)

 

Italian

21 (4.1)

 

Missing

0

SD: standard deviation; SCI: spinal cord injury.

Feelings: positive and negative affect

PA subscale. The Rasch analysis indicated a poor model fit (Tables III and IV). The standardized residual level of items 12 (alert), 14 (inspired), and 16 (determined) exceeded the critical level, but only the χ2 test of item 12 reached statistical significance. The t-tests indicated multidimensionality. Testing for local independency yielded no positive residual correlation coefficients. Reliability was very good (PSI = 0.89). Reversed thresholds were observed for items 5 (strong) and 12 (alert).

Table III. Rasch-based fit statistics for all measurement instruments, including initial and final analyses

Questionnaire and Items

(sample size for test of fit statistics)

Description of analysis

Person

Mean β

(95% CI)

Item

Mean δ

(95% CI)

χ2

df

p-value

t-test %

sig

(95% CI)

Reliability

(w/wou)

A, r

PANAS PA (n = 501)

Original scale

0.228

0.000

140.51

80

< 0.001

10.00

0.89, 0.88

(0.109, 0.347)

(–0.050, 0.050)

 

 

 

(8.10, 11.90)

 

 

2-Testlet analysis to correct for

signs of multidimensionality

0.183

0.000

2.52

18

0.99

3.41

0.81, 0.81

0.92, 0.91

(0.110, 0.256)

(–0.033, 0.033)

 

 

 

(1.44, 5.38)

 

 

PANAS NA (n = 470)

Original scale

0.228

0.000

140.51

80

< 0.001

10.00

0.89, 0.88

(0.109, 0.347)

(–0.041, 0.041)

 

 

 

(8.10, 11.90)

 

 

Combination of 2 response

options (items 6, 7, 13)

–1.622

0.000

108.22

70

0.002

4.81

0.77, 0.76

(–1.722, –1.511)

(-0.046, 0.046)

 

 

 

(2.90, 6.72)

 

 

ALE – Challenge (n = 459)

Original scale

–0.44

0.000

177.31

48

< 0.001

5.43

0.80, 0.78

(–0.535, –0.345)

(–0.037, 0.037)

 

 

 

(3.48, 7.38)

 

 

3-Testlet analysis to correct for

language DIF

–0.707

0.000

18.91

24

0.76

4.05

0.88, 0.86

1.10, 4.97

(–0.851, –0.563)

(–0.027, 0.027)

 

 

 

(2.17, 6.18)

 

 

ALE – Loss (n = 435)

Original scale

–0.675

0.000

63.34

36

0.004

1.89

0.74, 0.67

(–0.794, –0.556)

(–0.050, 0.050)

 

 

 

(0.07, 3.85)

 

 

ALE – Threat (n = 456)

Original scale

–0.897

0.000

112.98

48

< 0.001

2.09

0.78, 0.77

 

(–1.009, –0.785)

(–0.035, 0.035)

 

 

 

(0.14, 4.05)

 

 

2-Testlet analysis to correct for

signs of local dependency

–0.658

0.000

18.80

16

0.28

1.50

0.72, 0.72

0.92, 1.07

(–0.737, –0.579)

(–0.016, 0.016)

 

 

 

(0.00, 3.48)

 

 

PIL-SF (n = 478)

Original scale

1.75

0.000

34.99

32

0.33

4.79

0.85,.84

(1.57, 1.93)

(–0.036, 0.036)

 

 

 

(2.88, 6.70)

 

 

Combination of 2 response

options (item 3)

1.705

0.000

34.68

32

0.34

6.39

0.85, 0.84

(1.526, 1.884)

(–0.049, 0.049)

 

 

 

(4.48, 8.30)

 

 

BFI – Openness (n = 478)

Original scale

0.832

0.000

44.09

45

0.51

4.97

0.67, 0.59

(0.737, 0.927)

(–0.021, 0.021)

 

 

 

(3.07, 6.87)

 

 

Combination of 2 response

options (items 6, 7, 10)

0.90

0.000

30.02

35

0.71

3.98

0.65, 0.58

(0.799, 1.000)

(–0.027, 0.027)

 

 

 

(2.07, 5.88)

 

 

BFI – Conscientiousness (n = 475)

Original scale

1.040

0.000

24.32

24

0.44

2.40

0.61, 0.50

(0.935, 1.145)

(–0.017, 0.017)

 

 

 

(0.49, 4.30)

 

 

Combination of 2 response

options (items 2 and 16)

1.105

0.000

30.59

24

0.17

2.19

0.59, 0.48

(0.997, 1.213)

(–0.024, 0.024)

 

 

 

(0.28, 4.10)

 

 

BFI – Extraversion (n = 488)

Original scale

0.592

0.000

32.64

28

0.25

4.40

0.66, 0.60

(0.491, 0.693)

(–0.033, 0.033)

 

 

 

(2.49, 6.31)

 

 

Combination of 2 response

options for item 8

0.593

0.000

31.04

28

0.32

4.40

0.66, 0.60

(0.488, 0.700)

(–0.034, 0.034)

 

 

 

(2.49, 6.51)

 

 

BFI – Agreeableness (n = 491)

Original scale

1.60

0.000

103.01

32

< 0.001

1.60

0.53, 0.47

(0.00, 3.51)

(–0.014, 0.014)

 

 

 

(0.00, 3.51)

 

 

BFI – Neuroticism (n = 482)

Original scale

–0.734

0.000

53.06

32

0.01

3.61

0.73, 0.68

(–0.856, –0.612)

(–0.051, 0.051)

 

 

 

(1.70, 5.53)

 

 

Person mean β: person mean location in logits (beta); item mean δ: item mean location in logits (delta); χ2: chi-square based goodness of fit test; df: degrees of freedom; Reliability (w/wou): Person separation index (with extremes, without extremes), A: common variance between the testlets; r: association between the testlets.

 

Table IV. Raw scores and Rasch-based fit statistics, ordering of the response scale thresholds, local dependency and differential item functioning (DIF) of the Positive Affect Negative Affect Scale (PANAS) items

Questionnaire and Items

(sample size for test of fit statistics)

Item

Mean δ

(95% CI)

SE

z

χ2

df

p-value

τ

Local

dependency

DIF

PANAS Positive Affect – Full scale (n = 501)

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

PANAS Positive Affect 01 – interested

–0.692

0.068

–0.297

11.28

8

0.19

ord

Education

PANAS Positive Affect 03 – excited

0.242

0.059

1.081

2.37

8

0.97

ord

PANAS Positive Affect 05 – strong

0.719

0.058

–0.021

5.52

8

0.70

disord

PANAS Positive Affect 09 – enthusiastic

0.164

0.060

–2.480

13.28

8

0.10

ord

PANAS Positive Affect 10 – proud

0.490

0.057

1.931

7.18

8

0.52

ord

PANAS Positive Affect 12 – alert

–0.232

0.058

5.243

51.61

8

< 0.001

disord

Language

PANAS Positive Affect 14 – inspired

0.833

0.059

2.754

11.72

8

0.16

ord

PANAS Positive Affect 16 – determined

–0.319

0.060

–3.127

12.50

8

0.13

ord

PANAS Positive Affect 17 – attentive

–0.756

0.064

–0.779

9.49

8

0.30

ord

PANAS Positive Affect 19 – active

–0.449

0.060

–2.147

15.55

8

0.05

ord

PANAS Negative Affect – Full scale (n = 470)

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

PANAS Negative Affect 02 – distressed

–0.635

0.054

3.433

15.29

7

0.032

ord

Language

PANAS Negative Affect 04 – upset

–0.483

0.057

1.663

12.80

7

0.077

ord

PANAS Negative Affect 06 – guilty

0.490

0.067

–1.146

5.03

7

0.656

disord

PANAS Negative Affect 07 – scared

0.430

0.065

–2.567

17.54

7

0.014

disord

PANAS Negative Affect 08 – hostile

0.425

0.065

–1.181

9.54

7

0.216

ord

PANAS Negative Affect 11 – irritable

–0.392

0.060

–0.904

6.61

7

0.470

ord

PANAS Negative Affect 13 – ashamed

0.507

0.072

–0.223

4.04

7

0.775

disord

SCI severitya

PANAS Negative Affect 15 – nervous

–0.399

0.056

–0.312

10.22

7

0.176

ord

Language

PANAS Negative Affect 18 – jittery

–0.094

0.056

–1.076

6.30

7

0.505

ord

Language

PANAS Negative Affect 20 – afraid

0.151

0.059

–1.474

9.94

7

0.192

ord

Item mean δ: Item mean location in logits (delta); SE: standard error of item location; z: standard normal distributed test value z; df: degrees of freedom; τ: ordering of the response scale thresholds (tau); ord: ordered thresholds; disord: disordered thresholds; Local dependency using r ≥ 0.3 as criteria. aNon-uniform DIF.

Regarding targeting, the logit means of the 10 items were located within 1.6 logits. The item thresholds were spread along the whole logit continuum (Fig. 1A). Out of 503 participants with valid scores, 2 achieved the maximum raw total score, and none obtained the minimum score. The person and the item mean did not overlap, indicating a tendency toward a ceiling effect. We found uniform DIF for education (item 1) and language (item 12).

Due to signs of multidimensionality, we assigned the items according to their first PCA loadings to 2 groups and conducted additional Rasch analyses for each of the 2 groups. The Rasch analysis with the items 3, 5, 9 and 10 yielded a non-significant goodness of fit statistic, good fit for all items, and 2.81% significant t-tests. Reliability was very good (0.80). Item 5 had disordered thresholds and uniform sex DIF.

Rasch analysis of the second item subgroup (items 1, 12, 14, 16, 17, and 19) showed a borderline non-significant goodness of fit statistic, good item fits to the model, and 6.63% (95% CI [4.71, 8.54]) of significant t-tests. Reliability was very good (0.83). Thresholds of item 12 were disordered. No DIF was found.

Finally, to test whether the 2 item-groups form a unidimensional latent estimate, we conducted a testlet analysis, revealing that 92% of variance between the 2 item-groups was common, 8% uniquely factor-related (Table III). The goodness of fit statistics were good, thresholds ordered. We found a uniform education DIF for testlet 2, and uniform DIFs for both subtests for combined time and age since injury. To test for compensatory DIF we split testlet 1 by combined time and age since injury, and DIF on testlet 2 disappeared. Because the differences of the group mean locations were below 0.5 logits, hinting at inconsequential DIF, the item splits were not retained. Overall, items can be summated to form a unidimensional latent scale, but unique variance and DIF, inconsistently appearing at the item and testlet level, may bias the raw scores.

NA subscale. The Rasch analysis indicated poor model fit. The z-values of items 2 and 7 were above ± 2.50, but both χ2 tests were not significant (Table IV). The t-tests indicated unidimensionality. Testing for local independency did not reveal any positive residual correlation. Reliability was respectable (PSI = 0.77). Disordered thresholds were observed for items 6, 7 and 13.

The logit means of the 10 items were located within 1.1 logits. Many persons are located in the lower negative affect levels, which are not captured by many item thresholds (Fig. 1B). Considering all valid scores, 31 persons (6.19%) attained the minimum, no person the maximum raw total score. The person mean was below the item mean, overall indicating floor effects. Language DIF was detected for items 2, 15 and 18, a non-uniform DIF for SCI severity for item 13.

In an additional analysis we combined 2 response options for items 6, 7 and 13 to adjust for the disordered thresholds. The model fit remained statistically significant, the z-values of items 2 and 7 above ± 2.5, with a significant χ2 test for item 7. Thresholds were ordered, language DIF for items 2, 4, 15 and 18 were observed, but with mean locations between the groups for all class intervals below 0.5 logits indicating inconsequential DIF (Table III). Examining whether DIF caused item misfit or compensatory DIF by splitting item 2 on language, analysis yielded compensatory language DIF for item 18 and no item fit changes (results not shown).

 

15055.png
Fig. 1. Person-item threshold distribution for Positive Affect (A) and Negative Affect (B), extreme scores included.

Thoughts and beliefs: appraisals

Challenge. Rasch analysis revealed a poor model fit and items 6, 7 and 13 did not fit the Rasch model (Table V). The χ2 test for item 3 was not significant, but the z-value exceeded the critical level. The χ2 test for item 8 was statistically significant, but the z value was < 2.50. The pairwise t-tests supported unidimensionality. No positive residual correlations were observed, indicating local independency.

Table V. Raw scores and Rasch-based fit statistics, ordering of the response scale thresholds, local dependency and differential item functioning (DIF) of the Appraisal of Life Events Scale (ALE) items

Questionnaire and Items

(sample size for test of fit statistics)

Item

Mean δ

(95% CI)

SE

z

χ2

df

p-value

τ

Local

dependency

DIF

ALE Challenge – Full scale (n = 459)

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

ALE Challenge 3 – Enjoyable

0.259

0.039

4.705

18.22

8

0.020

disord

Education, administration mode

ALE Challenge 6 – Challenging

–0.526

0.042

5.290

53.19

8

< 0.001

ord

education

ALE Challenge 7 – Stimulating

0.143

0.044

–4.031

48.30

8

< 0.001

ord

Language

ALE Challenge 8 – Exhilarating

0.325

0.043

–1.804

22.73

8

0.004

ord

Language

ALE Challenge 12 – Informative

–0.504

0.044

–0.040

3.76

8

0.878

ord

ALE Challenge 13 – Exciting

0.303

0.043

–2.674

31.10

8

< 0.001

ord

ALE Loss – Full scale (n = 435)

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

ALE Loss 9 – Painful

–0.690

0.045

–0.435

8.90

9

0.446

ord

ALE Loss 10 – Depressing

–0.135

0.045

–1.511

20.36

9

0.016

ord

ALE Loss 11 – Pitiful

0.516

0.046

2.289

19.98

9

0.018

ord

ALE Loss 16 – Intolerable

0.309

0.046

–0.546

14.10

9

0.119

ord

ALE Threat – Full scale (n = 456)

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

ALE Threat 1 – Threatening

0.072

0.044

–0.306

12.27

8

0.139

ord

 

ALE Threat 2 – Fearful

–0.185

0.046

–1.417

19.85

8

0.011

ord

Education

ALE Threat 4 – Worrying

–0.607

0.048

2.065

7.56

8

0.477

ord

ALE Threat 5 – Hostile

0.125

0.043

3.513

36.26

8

< 0.001

disord

Language

ALE Threat 14 – Frightening

0.058

0.045

–1.403

19.98

8

0.010

ord

Yes

Language

ALE Threat 15 – Horrifying

0.538

0.047

–2.301

17.05

8

0.030

ord

Yes

Language

Item mean δ: Item mean location in logits (delta); SE: standard error of item location; z: standard normal distributed test value z; df: degrees of freedom; τ: ordering of the response scale thresholds (tau); ord: ordered thresholds; disord: disordered thresholds; Local dependency using r ≥ 0.3 as criteria.

 

Reliability was very good (PSI = 0.80). Thresholds were ordered, with exemption of item 3. Concerning targeting, the logit means of the 6 items were located within 1 logit. The item thresholds were spread, but slightly accumulated in the centre of the challenge continuum (Fig. 2A). Out of 485 valid scores, 23 persons (4.74%) attained the minimum, and no person the maximum raw total score. The person mean was below the item mean. Uniform DIF was observed for education (items 3 and 6), language (items 7 and 8), and administration mode (item 3).

To investigate whether language DIF caused item misfit or compensatory DIF, items 7 and 8 were split in an additional analysis. No changes in the item fits were observed. Education DIF for item 3 and 6 remained; language DIF for item 8 and administration mode DIF for item 3 were compensatory and disappeared (results not shown).

Alternatively, to examine whether language DIF cancels out at the latent test level, we conducted a testlet analysis based on the language DIF pattern (items with DIF with German > French; DIF with French > German; items with no DIF). All variance was common, but there was more dependence across the 3 subscales than within. Thresholds were ordered, 2 testlet fits were > 2.50, and we found non-significant χ2 tests (Table III). We found a uniform language DIF for 2 (representing items 7 and 8) of the 3 subtests. Again, splitting 1 subtest had no effect on the item fit, but made the DIF for the second testlet disappear (compensatory DIF). However, the differences of the testlet mean locations were below 0.5 logits for all class intervals, suggesting inconsequential DIF. Overall, items can be summated and capture Challenge on a latent level, but language versions may be inequivalent.

Loss. The goodness of fit statistic was statistically significant, indicating misfit to model expectations (Table III). All individual items fit to the model and no positive residual correlation was found (Table V). The t-tests indicated unidimensionality. Reliability was respectable (PSI = 0.74), and all thresholds ordered.

Regarding targeting, the logit means of the 4 items were located within 1.2 logits. Of the 486 valid scores, 44 persons (9.05%) reached the minimum, and 4 persons the maximum raw total score. The person mean was below the item mean, overall indicating a floor effect (Fig. 2B). No DIF was found.

Threat. The overall goodness of fit statistic was statistically significant, and item 5 did not fit the model (Table V). The pairwise t-tests supported unidimensionality. The residual correlation between items 14 and 15 was 0.30, indicating local dependency. Other coefficients were small and negative. The PSI was 0.78, indicating respectable reliability. The thresholds of item 5 were disordered.

Regarding targeting, the item thresholds were spread across the continuum, but many persons were located in the lower part (Fig. 2C). The person mean –0.90 (95%CI [–1.01, –0.79]) was located below the item mean. Of all 492 valid scores, 31 persons (6.30%) attained the minimum, no person attained the maximum raw total score, indicating a floor effect. Uniform DIF was found for education (item 2) and language (items 5, 14 and 15).

 

15088.png
Fig. 2. Person-item threshold distribution of the appraisals Challenge (A), Loss (B) and Threat (C), extreme scores included.

 

To further investigate local dependency, we built 2 testlets based on the loadings of the items on the first residual component. The common variance was 92% (Table III). We found disordered thresholds for 1 testlet (items 14 and 15), a z-value > 2.50 for the other testlet (χ2 test not significant), and a uniform language DIF for both testlets. Scrutinizing the mean locations between the groups for all class intervals hinted at inconsequential DIF. We then checked for compensatory DIF or effects of DIF on item fit. Splitting 1 testlet made DIF the second testlet disappear (compensatory DIF), and the testlets now fit the model (results not shown). Overall, items can be summated to capture a unidimensional latent Threat construct, but raw scores may be biased due to unique variance, local dependency, and language inequivalence.

Motives: purpose in life

The goodness of fit statistic of the PIL-SF was not significant (Table VI). The z-values of items 1 and 4 exceeded ± 2.50, but χ2 tests were not significant. The pairwise t-tests supported the unidimensionality assumption. All residual correlations were negative, indicating local independency. The reliability of the PIL-SF was very good (PSI = 0.85). Disordered thresholds were observed for item 3.

Table VI. Raw scores and Rasch-based fit statistics, ordering of the response scale thresholds, local dependency and differential item functioning (DIF) of the Purpose in Life test – Short Form (PIL-SF) items

Questionnaire and Items

(sample size for test of fit statistics)

Item mean δ

(95% CI)

SE

z

χ2

df

p-value

τ

Local

dependency

DIF

PIL-SF – Full scale (n = 478)

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

PIL-SF 1 – Presence of clear life goals

0.096

0.057

2.618

6.04

8

0.64

ord

PIL-SF 2 – Life being meaningful

–0.410

0.059

–0.738

7.98

8

0.44

ord

PIL-SF 3 – Life goal completion

0.521

0.060

0.669

4.39

8

0.82

disord

PIL-SF 4 – Presence of goals/life purpose

–0.207

0.061

–3.114

16.57

8

0.03

ord

Item mean δ: Item mean location in logits (delta); SE: Standard error of item location; z: Standard normal distributed test value z; df: Degrees of freedom; τ: Ordering of the response scale thresholds (tau); ord: ordered thresholds; disord: disordered thresholds; Local dependency using r ≥ 0.3 as criteria.

Regarding targeting, the item means were located within 1 logit. Item thresholds spread well across the PIL continuum (Fig. 3). Of 504 individuals with valid scores, 2 (0.40%) attained the minimum, and 23 (4.56%) the maximum raw total score. The mean person level lay above the mean item level, indicating a ceiling effect. We found no DIF. Collapsing 2 response options of item 3 in an additional analysis to adjust for the disordered thresholds did not change the findings of this measure (Table III).

 

15124.png
Fig. 3. Person-item threshold distribution along the Purpose in Life test – Short Form (PIL-SF) continuum, extreme scores included.

 

Patterns of experience and behaviour: Personality

Openness. The goodness of fit statistic was not significant, indicating good fit to the model (Table III). All individual items fit the Rasch model. The pairwise t-tests indicated unidimensionality (Table VII). The residual correlations indicated local independency, and no coefficient was positive. The PSI was 0.67, which points at minimally acceptable reliability. Thresholds were disordered for items 6, 7 and 10.

Table VII. Raw scores and Rasch-based fit statistics, ordering of the response scale thresholds, local dependency and differential item functioning (DIF) of the Big Five Inventory-K (BFI-K) items

Questionnaire and Items

(sample size for test of fit statistics)

Item mean δ

(95% CI)

SE

z

χ2

df

p-value

τ

Local

dependency

DIF

BIF-K Openness – Full scale (n = 478)

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

BIF-K Openness 6 – curious

–0.351

0.058

0.733

7.28

9

0.61

disord

BIF-K Openness 7 – ingenious

0.009

0.054

0.682

12.73

9

0.18

disord

Language

BIF-K Openness 10 – active imagination

–0.049

0.056

–0.111

5.65

9

0.77

disord

BIF-K Openness 15 –artistic experience

0.106

0.054

–0.447

11.14

9

0.27

ord

BIF-K Openness 21 – few artistic interest

0.285

0.048

0.704

7.29

9

0.61

ord

BFI-K Conscientiousness – Full scale (n = 475)

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

BFI-K Conscientiousness 2 – thorough job

–0.239

0.062

–0.673

8.80

6

0.19

disord

BFI-K Conscientiousness 13 – lazy

0.167

0.052

1.059

4.93

6

0.55

ord

BFI-K Conscientiousness 16 – efficient

–0.072

0.064

–1.198

7.28

6

0.30

disord

Language

BFI-K Conscientiousness 19 – follows plans

0.144

0.058

0.373

3.32

6

0.77

ord

BFI-K Extraversion – Full scale (n = 488)

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

BFI-K Extraversion 4 – reserved

0.516

0.053

0.183

6.72

7

0.46

ord

Language

BFI-K Extraversion 8 – generates enthusiasm

–0.108

0.057

0.537

7.59

7

0.37

disord

BFI-K Extraversion 11 – quiet

–0.039

0.053

0.552

10.62

7

0.16

ord

Language

BFI-K Extraversion 17 – outgoing

–0.369

0.060

0.430

7.71

7

0.36

ord

BFI-K Agreeableness – Full scale (491)

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

BFI-K Agreeableness 1 – fault in others

0.199

0.051

1.596

24.19

8

0.002

ord

Sex

BFI-K Agreeableness 12 – trusting

–0.022

0.053

2.617

23.47

8

< 0.001

ord

Language

BFI-K Agreeableness 14 – cold and aloof

0.002

0.050

–1.638

27.42

8

< 0.001

ord

 

BFI-K Agreeableness 18 – rude to others

–0.179

0.050

–1.782

28.01

8

< 0.001

ord

Language

BFI-K Neuroticism – Full scale (n = 482)

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

 

BFI-K Neuroticism 3 – depressed, blue

0.736

0.057

–1.348

14.50

8

0.07

ord

BFI-K Neuroticism 5 – relaxed

–0.219

0.059

2.798

11.60

8

0.17

ord

BFI-K Neuroticism 9 – worries a lot

–0.571

0.053

–0.579

11.41

8

0.18

ord

BFI-K Neuroticism 20 – nervous

0.028

0.056

–0.560

15.56

8

0.05

ord

Language

Item mean δ: Item mean location in logits (delta); SE: Standard error of item location; z: Standard normal distributed test value z; df: Degrees of freedom; τ: Ordering of the response scale thresholds (tau); ord: ordered thresholds; disord: disordered thresholds; Reliability (w/wou): Local dependency using r ≥ 0.3 as criteria.

 

Regarding targeting, the logit means of all items were located within 0.6 logits. The person mean was located above the item mean. The item thresholds accumulated in approximately 3 areas of the continuum, at –1, 0 and 1.5 logits. No thresholds were located in higher areas (Fig. 4A). Of all 503 valid scores, no person attained the minimum, but 25 persons (4.97%) attained the maximum raw total score. Overall, this finding points to a marginal ceiling effect.

We identified a uniform language DIF for item 7. However, it disappeared after combining 2 response options for items 6, 7 and 10 in an additional analysis (Table IV).

Conscientiousness. The goodness of fit statistic was non-significant and all items showed fit to the model. Pairwise t-tests pointed at unidimensionality. No positive residual correlation was observed, indicating local independency. Reliability was low, with PSI = 0.61 (Table VII). Disordered thresholds were found for items 2 and 16. Regarding targeting, item means were located within 0.4 logits. The person mean was located above the item mean. The item thresholds were spread along the conscientiousness continuum, with a slight tendency towards lower levels (Fig. 4B). No person attained the minimum, but 28 persons (5.56%) attained the maximum raw total score, suggesting a ceiling effect. Uniform language DIF was identified for item 16.

In an additional analysis 2 response options for items 2 and 6 were collapsed (Table III), which yielded good fit statistics. Language DIF for item 16 persisted, but the mean locations between the groups for all class intervals hinted at inconsequential DIF.

Extraversion. The goodness of fit statistic was not statistically significant, and all items fit the model. The pairwise t-tests hinted at unidimensionality. No positive residual correlation was found, indicating local independency (Table VII).

Reliability was minimally acceptable, with PSI = 0.66. Disordered thresholds were found for item 8. Regarding targeting, the item means were located within 1 logit. The person mean was situated on a higher extraversion level than the item mean. The thresholds are spread along the extraversion continuum, with missing thresholds between –1 and 0, and 1 and 2 (Fig. 4C). Of 503 valid scores, 1 person (0.20%) attained the minimum, and 13 (2.58%) the maximum raw total score, indicating a tendency towards a ceiling effect. We found a uniform language DIF for items 4 and 11. In a further analysis we collapsed 2 response options of item 8 (Table III). Language DIF for items 4 and 11 persisted, but the mean locations between the groups for all class intervals indicated inconsequential DIF.

Agreeableness. The goodness of fit statistic was statistically significant (Table III). The z-value of item 12 was above 2.50, with a non-significant χ2 test. In contrast, the standardized residual levels of the other items were below ± 2.50, but statistically significant. The pairwise t-tests supported unidimensionality. Residual correlations were negative, indicating local independency. Reliability was unacceptable, with PSI = 0.53. All item thresholds were ordered.

Concerning targeting, the item means were spread along 0.4 logits. The person mean’s location was higher than the item mean. The item thresholds (Fig. 4D) are spread along the agreeableness continuum, but are not located in the highest agreeableness levels. The number of persons reaching the maximum raw total score was 12 (2.39%). No persons attained the minimum raw total score, overall, suggesting a ceiling effect.

Examination of DIF revealed a uniform sex DIF for item 1 and a uniform language DIF for items 12 and 18 (Table VII), however, with mean locations between the groups for all class intervals below 0.5 logits. To test for compensatory DIF and whether DIF caused item misfit we split the DIF items (item 1 for sex; item 18 for language). No improvement of model or item fit was observed, but language DIF for item 12 disappeared (results not shown).

Neuroticism. Rasch analysis resulted in a significant goodness of fit statistic (Table III). The z-value of item 5 was above ± 2.50, but the χ2 test was not statistically significant. Pairwise t-tests pointed at unidimensionality. No positive residual correlation was found, indicating local independency (Table VII). Reliability was respectable, with PSI = 0.73. The thresholds of all items were ordered.

Regarding targeting, the 4 item means were spread along 1.3 logits. The person mean was below the item mean. The item thresholds were spread along the neuroticism continuum, but no threshold was located between –1 and –2 logits (Fig. 4E). Out of 502 valid scores, 18 persons (3.59%) attained the minimum, and 1 person the maximum raw total score, suggesting a floor effect.

Examination of DIF revealed a uniform language DIF for item 20, but the mean locations between the groups for all class intervals of item 20 pointed at inconsequential DIF. Splitting item 20 did not improve overall model fit or fit of item 5 (results not shown).

Fig. 4. Person-item threshold distribution of the 5 person­ality dimensions (A) openness, (B) conscientiousness, (C) extraversion, (D) agreeableness, and (E) neuroticism, extreme scores included.

15155.png

 

DISCUSSION

This study examined the psychometric qualities of 4 PPF using a modern, test-theoretical approach with Rasch analysis. Indications for multi-dimensionality were found for the Positive Affect items. All other scales represent unidimensional measures. Reliability ranged from adequate to very good, except for the BFI-K agreeableness and extraversion subscales. The PPF measures are suitable for an SCI population, indications for bias by SCI-related characteristics were found for just 1 item across all scales. However, with the exception of the PIL-SF, we repeatedly found language DIF. Language DIF can be accommodated within the Rasch framework, but the raw sum scores of the different language versions may not be equivalent, even though our results indicated inconsequential impacts of DIF.

Our analyses of the PANAS-PA subscale suggested a bi-dimensional factor structure. The 2 sub-dimensions could stand for positive self-view (items 3, 5, 9 and 10), and motivation-related alertness with a future outlook (items 1, 12, 14, 16, 17 and 19). Our findings, however, do not correspond with a recent study applying the Rasch methodology using Winsteps software with the Brazilian Portuguese questionnaire version, as no multidimensionality was found (47). Multi-dimensionality could bias raw total scores, since the score may not capture the intended construct. Our analyses suggest that items can be summated to form a unidimensional latent scale using a bi-factor approach. This approach, however, needs to be applied when comparing total scores across populations.

In contrast to PA, our results support unidimensionality for NA. The observed floor effect indicates that our study participants reported low levels of NA. From a psychological perspective, this finding is pleasing, hinting at good well-being of our SCI study sample (48). From a psychometric point of view, it means that the NA items might not have captured distinct patterns in the low-NA levels, which could be covered with additional NA-items. In the context of SwiSCI a clear differentiation of high NA levels seems preferable, potentially representing a proxy for complications in the adjustment to SCI. High scorers on negative affect may need a more detailed clarification of their mental health. Test equating, i.e. putting the NA items on a common metric with mental health measures with a clinical cut-off score, may provide more knowledge on potential, clinically meaningful cut-off-scores for the NA-subscale.

We found unidimensionality and very good reliability for the PIL-SF. Validated versions of the French and Italian PIL-SFs did not exist prior to SwiSCI. The forms were translated in a forward translation procedure. Collapsing response options, as for item 3, reduces the range of the raw total score and impedes comparability across studies. However, since only 2 responses of 1 item needed collapsing, the impact on the PIL-SF total score range (4–28) is small.

Our findings regarding the 3 subscales of the ALE diverge dramatically. The Loss subscale showed unidimensionality, respectable reliability, and invariance across all language versions. In contrast, for both Challenge and Threat, testlets had to be created and language bias persisted even at the testlet level, although in a range indicating inconsequential impact. Standardized French and Italian versions of the ALE did not exist and were translated by our study team. A closer examination of these language versions in other settings is warranted, along with a careful inspection of the items in each context.

The positive side in relation to the 5 personality subscales of the BFI-K is that unidimensionality was revealed. The 5 personality characteristics represent comprehensive, broad constructs, which is why lower reliability estimates could be expected. Nonetheless, the low PSI value for agreeableness and conscientiousness indicated unreliable assessment of these constructs. Taking 0.80 as the cut-off for reliable group comparisons reveals insufficient reliability for all 5 subscales, and hence questionable usability within population studies (40).

The frequently observed language DIFs observed in most measurement instruments could reflect cultural differences across the Swiss-German-, French-, and Italian-speaking populations, or differences in item phrasings. For example, the French BFI-K item 16 (efficient) of the conscientiousness subscale refers to the work context (efficient in work), while the German item alludes to a more general statement of being efficient when working, which can comprise more aspects than work per se. Overall, a careful re-examination of the DIF items might be warranted. This examination is especially important in the context of an epidemiological survey, such as SwiSCI, which aims to describe health and quality of life and the potential risk factors within a specified multi-lingual population. Comparisons across language groups, but also comparisons with the general population, may yield biased results. Although transformation tables have been offered to convert raw total scores to a logic metric scale and adjust for biased effects (e.g. 49), cross-validations with larger sample sizes are needed, and we refrain from premature disclosure of transformed logit scores.

Our study is subject to several limitations. First, important characteristics of a measurement instrument, such as test-retest reliability and sensitivity to change, were not tested due to the cross-sectional study design. Secondly, a larger sample is needed to cross-validate our findings regarding DIF. The sample sizes per language group were different, but representative for the Swiss context, i.e. multiple national languages in the same country.

The current study has shown that a majority of our measures of PPF satisfy the assumption of unidimensionality of the Rasch model. The Swiss context represents a major challenge from a psychometric point of view because equivalence across languages is not guaranteed. Comparisons with the English original measures were not part of this study, but should be addressed in the future. The validation of measures by the use of modern test-theoretical approaches, such as the Rasch model, should become a regular step within epidemiological studies.

Funding

This study was funded by the Swiss Paraplegic Research (SPF), Nottwil, Switzerland.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

This study has been financed in the framework of the Swiss Spinal Cord Injury Cohort Study (SwiSCI, www.swisci.ch), supported by the Swiss Paraplegic Association.

The members of the SwiSCI Steering Committee are: Olivier Dériaz (Clinique Romande de Réadaptation, Sion); Michael Baumberger and Hans Peter Gmünder (Swiss Paraplegic Center, Nottwil); Armin Curt and Martin Schubert (University Clinic Balgrist, Zürich); Kerstin Hug and Margret Hund-Georgiadis (REHAB Basel, Basel); Hans Georg Koch and Urs Styger (Swiss Paraplegic Association, Nottwil); Hardy Landolt (representative for persons with SCI, Glarus); Rita Schaumann-Von Stosch (SUVA, Luzern); Mirjam Brach and Gerold Stucki (Swiss Paraplegic Research, Nottwil); and Martin Brinkhof and Christine Thyrian (SwiSCI Study Center at Swiss Paraplegic Research, Nottwil).

The authors would like to thank study participants and the personnel of the SwiSCI study centre.

REFERENCES

1. Bickenbach JOA, Shakespeare T, von Groote P, editors. International perspectives on spinal cord injury. Geneva: Word Health Organization; 2013.

2. Bonanno GA, Kennedy P, Galatzer-Levy IR, Lude P, Elfstrom ML. Trajectories of resilience, depression, and anxiety following spinal cord injury. Rehabil Psychol 2012; 57: 236–247.

3. Hobfoll SE. Social and psychological resources and adaptation. Rev Gen Psychol 2002; 6: 307–324.

4. Peter C, Müller R, Cieza A, Geyh S. Psychological resources in spinal cord injury: a systematic literature review. Spinal Cord 2012; 50: 188–201.

5. van Leeuwen CM, Kraaijeveld S, Lindeman E, Post MW. Associations between psychological factors and quality of life ratings in persons with spinal cord injury: a systematic review. Spinal Cord 2012; 50: 174–187.

6. Rose A, Piatt JA, Zahl M, Kim K. The effect of a self-efficacy based forum on life satisfaction for individuals with spinal cord injury or disease. Ann Therapeut Recreat 2008; 16: 49–56.

7. Zahl ML, Compton DM, Kim K, Rosenbluth JP. SCI/D forum to increase active living: The effect of a self-efficacy and self-affirmation based SCI/D forum on active living in adults with spinal cord injury/disease. SCI Psychosoc Process 2008; 21: 5–13.

8. Kennedy P, Taylor N, Hindson L. A pilot investigation of a psychosocial activity course for people with spinal cord injuries. Psychol Health Med 2006; 11: 91–99.

9. Geyh S, Müller R, Peter C, Bickenbach J, Post MWM, Stucki G, et al. Capturing the psychologic-personal perspective in spinal cord injury. Am J Phys Med Rehabil 2011; 90: S79–S96.

10. Rasch G. Probabilistic models for some intelligence and attainment tests. Copenhagen: Danish Institute for Educational Research; 1960.

11. Embretson SE, Reise SP. Item response theory for psychologists. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2000.

12. Tennant A, Conaghan PG. The Rasch measurement model in rheumatology: what is it and why use it? When should it be applied, and what should one look for in a Rasch Paper? Arthritis Care Res 2007; 57: 1358–1362.

13. Tennant A, Pallant JF. Unidimensionality matters! (A tale of two Smiths?.) Rasch Meas Transact 2006; 20: 1048–1051.

14. Watson D, Clark LA, Tellegen A. Development and validation of brief measures of positive and negative affect. The PANAS scale. J Pers Soc Psychol 1988; 54: 1063–1070.

15. Ferguson E, Matthews G, Cox T. The Appraisal of Life Events (ALE) scale: reliability and validity. Br J Health Psychol 1999; 4: 97–116.

16. Schulenberg SE, Schnetzer LW, Buchanan EM. The Purpose in Life Test-Short Form: development and psychometric support. J Happiness Stud 2011; 12: 861–876.

17. Rammstedt B, John OP. Kurzversion des Big Five Inventory (BFI-K): Entwicklung und Validierung eines ökonomischen Inventars zur Erfassung der fünf Faktoren der Persönlichkeit. [Short version of the Big Five Inventory (BFI-K): Development and validation of an economical inventory to assess the five factors of personality.] Diagnostica 2005; 51: 195–206.

18. Post MW, Brinkhof MW, von Elm E, Boldt C, Brach M, Fekete C, et al. Design of the Swiss Spinal Cord Injury Cohort Study. Am J Phys Med Rehabil 2011; 90: S5–S16.

19. Terracciano A, McCrae RR, Costa PTJ. Factorial and construct validity of the Italian Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS). Eur J Psychol Assess 2003; 19: 131–141.

20. Krohne HW, Egloff B, Kohlmann C-W, Tausch A. Untersuchung mit einer deutschen Form der Positive and Negative Affect Schedule (PANAS). Diagnostica 1996; 42: 139–156.

21. Gaudreau P, Sanchez X, Blondin J-P. Positive and Negative Affective States in a performance-related setting: testing the factorial structure of the PANAS across two samples of French-Canadian participants. Eur J Psychol Assess 2006; 22: 240–249.

22. Zevon MA, Tellegen A. The structure of mood change: an idiographic/nomothetic analysis. J Pers Soc Psychol 1982; 43: 111–122.

23. Gourounti K, Anagnostopoulos F, Vaslamatzis G. Primary appraisal of infertility: Evaluation of the psychometric properties of a Greek version of the Appraisal of Life Events Scale (ALE) in a sample of infertile women undergoing fertility treatment. Women Health 2010; 50: 688–704.

24. Crumbaugh JC, Maholick LT. An experimental study in existentialism: the psychometric approach to Frankl’s concept of noogenic neurosis. J Clin Psychol 1964; 20: 200–207.

25. Hauser J. Vom Sinn des Leidens: die Bedeutung systemtheoretischer, existenzphilosophischer und religiös-spiritueller Anschauungsweisen für die therapeutische Praxis. Würzburg, Germany: Königshausen & Neumann GmbH; 2004.

26. Schnetzer LW, Schulenberg SE, Buchanan EM. Differential associations among alcohol use, depression and perceived life meaning in male and female college students. J Subst Use 2013; 18: 311–319.

27. Drescher CF, Baczwaski BJ, Walters AB, Aiena BJ, Schulenberg SE, Johnson LR. Coping with an ecological disaster: the role of perceived meaning in life and self-efficacy following the Gulf oil spill. Ecopsychology 2012; 4: 56–63.

28. John OP, Donahue EM, Kentle RL. The “Big Five” inventory – versions 4a and 54. Berkeley, CA: University of California, Berkeley, Institute of Personality and Social Research; 1991.

29. Fossati A, Borroni S, Marchione D, Maffei C. The Big Five Inventory (BFI) Reliability and validity of its Italian Translation in three independent nonclinical samples. Eur J Psychol Assess 2011; 27: 50–58.

30. Plaisant O, Srivastava S, Mendelsohn GA, Debray Q, John OP. Relations entre le Big Five Inventory français et le manuel diagnostique des troubles mentaux dans un échantillon clinique français. Annales Médico Psychologiques 2005; 163: 161–167.

31. Wright BD, Masters GN. Rating scale analysis. Chicago, IL: MESA; 1982.

32. Andrich D, Sheridan BS, Luo G. Rumm 2030: Rasch Unidimensional measurement models [computer software]. 2010; Perth, Western Australia: RUMM Laboratory; 2010.

33. Andrich D. Rasch models for measurement. Newbury Park, CA: Sage; 1988.

34. Smith EV, Jr. Detecting and evaluating the impact of multidimensionality using item fit statistics and principal component analysis of residuals. J Appl Meas 2002; 3: 205–231.

35. Wainer H, Kiely G. Item clusters and computer adaptive testing: a case for testlets. J Educ Meas 1987; 24: 185–202.

36. Baghaei P. Local dependency and Rasch measures. Rasch Meas Transact 2008; 21: 1105–1106.

37. Wright BD. Local dependency, correlations and principal components. Rasch Meas Transact 1996; 10: 509–511.

38. Andrich D. Components of variance of scales with a subscale structure using two calculations of coefficient alpha. (Pensamiento Educativo) Journal of Latin American Educational Research 2015; In Press.

39. DeVellis RF. Scale Development: theory and applications. 3rd edn. Thousand Oaks, California: SAGE Publications, Inc.; 2012.

40. Nunally JC, Bernstein IH. Psychometric theory. 3rd edn. New York: McGraw-Hill; 1994.

41. Linacre JM. Optimizing rating scale category effectiveness. J Appl Meas 2002; 3: 85–106.

42. Bond TG, Fox CM. Applying the Rasch model: fundamental measurement in the human sciences. Mahwah, NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 2001.

43. Bland JM, Altman DG. Multiple significance tests: the Bonferroni method. Br Med J 1995; 310: 170.

44. Andrich D, Hagquist C. Real and artificial differential item functioning. J Educ Behav Stat 2012; 37: 387–416.

45. Tennant A, Pallant JF. DIF matters: a practical approach to test if differential item functioning makes a difference. Rasch Meas Transact 2007; 20: 1082–1084.

46. Wright BD, Panchapakesan N. A procedure for sample-free item analysis. Educ Psychol Meas 1969; 29: 23–48.

47. Pires P, Filgueiras A, Ribas R, Santana C. Positive and Negative Affect Schedule: psychometric properties for the Brazilian Portuguese version. Span J Psychol 2013; 16: 1–9.

48. Geyh S, Kunz S, Müller R, Peter C. Describing functioning and health after spinal cord injury in the light of psychological-personal factors. J Rehabil Med 2016; 48: 219–234.

49. Lundgren-Nilsson A, Jonsdottir IH, Ahlborg G, Jr, Tennant A. Construct validity of the Psychological General Well Being Index (PGWBI) in a sample of patients undergoing treatment for stress-related exhaustion: a Rasch analysis. Health Qual Life Outcomes 2013; 11: 2.

50. Zigmond AS, Snaith RP. The Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale. Acta Psychiatr Scand 1983; 67: 361–370.

51. Muller R, Cieza A, Geyh S. Rasch analysis of the Hospital Anxiety and Depression Scale in spinal cord injury. Rehabil Psychol 2012; 57: 214–223.

52. WHOQOL Group. Development of the World Health Organization WHOQOL-BREF quality of life assessment. The WHOQOL Group. Psychol Med 1998; 28: 551–558.

53. Geyh S, Fellinghauer BAG, Kirchberger I, Post MWM. Cross-cultural validity of four quality of life scales in persons with spinal cord injury. Health Qual Life Outcomes 2010; 3: 94.

54. Carver CS. You want to measure coping but your protocol’s too long: consider the Brief COPE. Int J Behav Med 1997; 4: 92–100.

55. Riggio RE, Canary DR. Social skills inventory manual (2nd edn). Redwood City, CA: MindGarden; 2003.

56. Schwarzer R, Jerusalem M. Generalized Self-Efficacy scale. In: Weinman J, Wright S, Johnston M, editors. Measures in health psychology: a user’s portfolio causal and control beliefs. Windsor, UK: NFER-NELSON; 1995, p. 35–37.

57. Peter C, Cieza A, Geyh S. Rasch analysis of the General Self-Efficacy Scale in spinal cord injury. J Health Psychol 2013; 19: 544–555.

Comments

Do you want to comment on this paper? The comments will show up here and if appropriate the comments will also separately be forwarded to the authors. You need to login/create an account to comment on articles. Click here to login/create an account.