Content » Vol 97, Issue 2

Investigative Report

A Test–Retest Reliability Study of Human Experimental Models of Histaminergic and Non-histaminergic Itch

Hjalte H. Andersen1, Anne-Kathrine R. Sørensen1, Gebbie A. R. Nielsen1, Marianne S. Mølgaard1, Pernille Stilling1, Shellie A. Boudreau1, Jesper Elberling2 and Lars Arendt-Nielsen1

1SMI®, Department of Health science and Technology, Faculty of Medicine, Aalborg University, Aalborg, and 2The Allergy Clinic, Copenhagen University Hospital, Gentofte, Copenhagen, Denmark

ABSTRACT

Numerous exploratory, proof-of-concept and interventional studies have used histaminergic and non-histaminergic human models of itch. However, no reliability studies for such surrogate models have been conducted. This study investigated the test–retest reliability for the response to histamine- and cowhage- (5, 15, 25 spiculae) induced itch in healthy volunteers. Cowhage spiculae were individually applied with tweezers and 1% histamine was applied with a skin prick test (SPT) lancet, both on the volar forearm. The intensity of itch was recorded on a visual analogue scale and self-reported area of itch was assessed 5 and 10 min after itch provocation. Reliability of the evoked itch (area under the curve and peak intensity) was assessed by the coefficient of variation (CV), intra-class correlation coefficient (ICC), and sample size estimation for parallel and cross-over designs. Cowhage (ICC = 0.57–0.77, CVbetween = 97%, CVwithin = 41%) and histamine: (ICC = 0.83–0.93, CVbetween = 97%, CVwithin = 20%) exhibited moderate-to-excellent intra-individual reliability and moderate inter-individual reliability for the itch intensity. For a test–retest observation period of one week, SPT-delivered histamine and application of cowhage-spiculae are reproducible human models of itch. The high inter-individual and low intra-individual variability suggests cross-over designed studies when applicable.

Key words: itch; cowhage; histamine; reliability; itch models; test–retest.

Accepted Jul 4, 2016; Epub ahead of print Jul 5, 2016

Acta Derm Venereol 2017; 97: XX–XX.

Corr: Lars Arendt-Nielsen, Department of Health Science and Technology, Faculty of Medicine, Aalborg University, Fredrik Bajers Vej 7D3, DK-9220 Aalborg E, Denmark. E-mail: LAN@hst.aau.dk

INTRODUCTION

Itch, also known as pruritus, is defined as “an unpleasant sensation evoking a desire to scratch” (1, 2). Chronic itch (> 6 weeks (3)) is highly bothersome and is usually a manifestation of a disease, but can also occur without any obvious somatic causes. Chronic itch is often associated with decreased quality of life, insomnia, and attentional difficulties, and, in this respect, is similar to the well-known consequences of chronic pain (2, 4, 5). Neurophysiologically, itch is conveyed by at least 2 separate pathways; histaminergic itch is transmitted by nociceptive mechano-insensitive C-fibres (CMi) responsive to, for example, histamine, while non-histaminergic itch is transmitted by polymodal nociceptive mechano-sensitive C-fibres (PmC) responsive to, for example, mucunain from cowhage (6–8).

Histamine, which is by far the most studied pruritogen, excites the subset of pruriceptive CMi-fibres expressing H1R and H4R. CMi-fibres are peptidergic, terminate within and below the stratum granulosum of the epidermis, and have large receptive fields. As a result the activation of CMi-fibres is usually associated with the neurogenic axon-reflex (flare) (1, 2, 4, 9). Histaminergic itch, e.g. in chronic urticaria, is generally responsive to antihistamines. However, a number of disorders causing chronic itch cannot be treated with antihistamines, underscoring the clinical importance of also understanding the non-histaminergic pathway of itch (7, 10–13). A distinct non-histaminergic pathway of itch is mediated by the protease-activated receptors (PAR2 and PAR4) and transmitted through PmC- and Aδ-fibres with smaller receptive fields than CMi-fibres (14). PAR2 is the prototypical human receptor associated with non-histaminergic itch (10, 15, 16). Upon binding of a ligand, either exogenously or endogenously, a cleavage of PAR2 occurs, as well as a co-activation of transient receptor potential anillin 1 (2, 10, 17, 18). The most frequently studied exogenous PAR2-agonist is mucunain, a cysteine protease found on the spiculae of the plant Mucuna pruriens, commonly known as cowhage. Cowhage spiculae activate the subset of pruriceptive fibres leading to the perception of both itch and mild pricking/stinging pain (3, 8, 13). The co-activation of Aδ-fibres should conceivable result in better spatial resolution and localizability of the non-histaminergic itch compared with histaminergic itch (7, 19–23), but this has not been confirmed. This could theoretically parallel the findings in the area of pain, where Aδ-fibre-mediated pricking pain is well localized, whereas C-fibre-mediated burning pain is more diffuse (19–23). Pruriceptive primary afferents of both the histaminergic and non-histaminergic itch pathway are multimodal, in the sense that they are also capable of transmitting nociceptive signals in response to algogenic stimuli, such as capsaicin (24).

The methods for induction of histamine- and cowhage-induced itch have been based on numerous different application techniques (8, 15, 25), but very little is known about their test–retest reliability. Reliable, symptom- and mechanism-specific human and animal surrogate models are increasingly applied in biomedical research for several different purposes: (i) they provide a convenient, mechanism-based approach to profiling novel drug candidates in early clinical development; (ii) they can be used as provocation modalities to probe relevant patient populations for explorative, segmentational or diagnostic purposes or to test potential peripheral and central sensitization phenomena; and (iii) they can be readily applied for testing neurophysiological aspects of particular sensory symptomatology in animals and humans (3–5, 13, 26–30). Human surrogate itch models can also be applied to effectively guide the choice of the clinical study population in clinical trials, increasing the likelihood of a successful drug development (3, 5, 31). The present study aimed to assess the test–retest reliability of histamine- and cowhage-induced itch, together with an assessment of potential differences in the spatial discrimination of itch evoked by cowhage and histamine.

MATERIALS AND METHODS

Two 1.5 h study sessions were performed, approximately one week apart, on the same group of male volunteers. The order of itch provocations with cowhage and histamine, as well as arm dominancy was randomized. This order of itch provocation was established in the first experimental session and repeated in the second session. While the subjects were unaware of which itch provocations were being applied and were instructed to look away during the administration, unblinding could have occurred based on the different tactile sensations evoked during application. The regional ethics committee approved the experimental protocol (approval number N-20140078) and the experiments were conducted at the Laboratory of Experimental Cutaneous Pain, Center for Sensory-Motor Interaction, Aalborg University in accordance with the principles of the Declaration of Helsinki (32). All participants provided written and oral informed consent prior to study participation and were informed that they could withdraw from the study, at any time.

Subjects

Fifteen male caucasian subjects (age 24.07 ± 0.73 years) participated in the study. Inclusion criteria were healthy male subjects, in the age range 20–35 years. Exclusion criteria were: any history of neurological, musculoskeletal, mental, dermatological, or allergic disorders, as well as any other on-going itch or pain problems.

Elicitation of itch with cowhage and histamine

Itch was induced in predefined application areas of the volar forearms of each subject. Three different arrangements of cowhage, using 5, 15, and 25 spiculae, as well as an application of histamine (1%) (Default Preparation, Diagenics, Bletchley, UK), were tested and compared (Fig. 1A). Application of the cowhage spiculae and histamine was randomized between the right and left arm. The 4 areas used for cowhage and histamine application were next to the middle of the volar forearms measured from the wrist to the cubital fossa (Fig. 1B). Using this demarcation, one application area was marked 2.5 cm above the middle of the forearm and one area was marked 2.5 cm below. The 4 areas measured 1.5×1.5 cm and were marked with a metallic paint marker (054, Edding International GmbH, Ahrensburg, Germany). Cowhage spiculae were prepared immediately prior to application under a stereo-microscope (Seben Incognita, Seben GmbH, Berlin, Germany) using a negative-grip tweezer (Electron Microscopy Science, Dumont, Switzerland) to arrange and transfer spiculae to another set of up to 5 insertion tweezers (Wittex GmbH, Munich, Germany). Each of the prepared tweezers was loaded with 5 spiculae, approximately 1 mm apart, which permitted the insertion of 4 (±1) spiculae in the skin per tweezer. The spiculae were inserted in the skin (Fig. 1B) within a 30 s interval and at an angle of approximately 45°, as described previously (33). The subjects did not observe the application of cowhage spicules or the skin prick test (SPT) lancet introduction of histamine. After 15 min the spiculae were removed with tape. A microdermascope (DinoLite, AnMo Electronics Corporation, Naarden, The Netherlands) was used to capture magnified images of the cowhage spiculae applied to the skin, and to validate that an acceptable amount of spiculae had been inserted successfully (±20% of intended spiculae). The images were analysed with the proprietary manufacturer software (DinoCapture 2.0, AnMo Electronics Corporation).


Fig. 1. (A) Positioning of the application areas for itch induction with cowhage and histamine on the volar forearm. (B) Approximate placement of the cowhage spiculae and histamine in each application area. In area 4 a droplet of histamine was applied and introduced intra-epidermally using a lancet prick. Each area measured 1.5×1.5 cm.

Histamine was applied with an SPT lancet (Allergopharma, Diagenics, Bletchley, UK) and a pressure of 120 g, using a SENSEBox electronic von Frey transducer (SENSEBox EvF, Somedic AB, Hörby, Sweden), with an attached custom-made prick lancet mount (Aalborg University, Aalborg, Denmark). The controlled pressure applied was aimed at increasing reliability and based on an initial pilot study showing that 120 g was sufficient to establish a wheal > 3 mm in diameter in all participants, while minimizing the occurrence of bleeding (34, 35). Histamine application using weight-controlled SPT was chosen as a comparator, instead of, for example, iontophoresis, intradermal injections or inactivated histamine-coated spiculae for several reasons: (i) SPT application of histamine has been reported to produce a stronger and more lasting itch sensation than iontophoresis and is associated with less pain and discomfort than intradermal injections (36, 37); (ii) it is the most commonly studied approach, while being easy, safe and clinically applicable (e.g. compared with coated spiculae and intradermal injections) (38); (iii) it requires a minimum of equipment and thus the method can easily be applied in any setting in future studies; (iv) compared with intradermal injection and iontophoresis, SPT application resembles that of spiculae, in that the applied substance is mainly brought into contact with the epidermis and the dermoepidermal junction in a very localized manner.

Outcome measures

Assessment of experimentally-induced itch and pain intensity. The intensity of the elicited itch was assessed every 20 s for 10 min, from the moment of application, using a computerized visual analogue scale (VAS). The VAS ranged from 0 = ”no itch” to 10 = ”worst imaginable itch” with a label at 50% of its length, labelled “strong urge to scratch”. Similar VAS modifications were used in the studies by Mørch et al. (39) and Hartmann et al. (13). The subjects were instructed to rate the sensation of itch by using the 50% mark as a guideline. Simultaneously to itch rating, the pain intensity of 2 sensory descriptors “pricking/stinging” and “burning” were reported for 10 min using 2 similar VASs with a sampling rate of 1/min. The VAS intensity for “pricking/stinging” allowed ratings from 0=”no pricking/stinging” to 10=”worst imaginable pricking/stinging pain”. Likewise, the VAS for the burning pain allowed ratings from 0 = ”no burning” to 10 = “worst imaginable burning pain”. Both scales for nociceptive sensory qualities had identical 50% marks labelled “strongly painful sensation” analogue to the 50% label on the scale applied for itch sensation.

Localization and spatial distribution of induced itch. Navigate Pain software (Aalborg University, Aalborg, Denmark) was used to record the location and area of itch experienced by each subject 5 and 10 min after each itch provocation. This method has previously been validated and comparatively analysed matched to traditional hard-copy pain drawings in pain patients and healthy controls (40). The subjects were specifically instructed to: (i) draw the area where they felt itch on a 3D body schema representing the left or right volar forearm similar to the arm schema (Fig. 1A), (ii) disregard the area where the spiculae or the histamine had been applied which was visually observable during the VAS recordings, and (iii) focus only on the perceived area of itch. The percentage of the drawn area vs. the total arm area was multiplied by 2 to account for the fact that the applied body schema showed both volar forearms. Finally, the spatial distribution was obtained by superimposing the perceived areas of itch for each of the 4 itch provocations, at 5 and 10 min.

Statistical analysis

The software SPSS Statistics version 22 (IBM corporation, Redmond, USA) was used to analyse all data and to assess statistical significance. A significance level at p ≤ 0.05 was chosen, marked with (*) in the results section, while highly significant differences with p ≤ 0.01 are marked with (**). Descriptive parameters; arithmetic mean and standard error of the mean (SEM) were calculated for all reported parameters and are shown in the results section unless specifically stated otherwise. Visual inspection and confirmatory Shapiro-Wilk’s tests were performed to ascertain the normality of the data. For all parameters recorded with the VAS, the area under the curve (AUC) was calculated and the peak itch intensity was noted for every subject in response to each of the 4 itch provocations (5, 15, 25 spiculae and 1% histamine) and at each session. The itch AUC, the peak itch intensity and the perceived area of itch from the 2 sessions were tested by 2 repeated measures analysis of variance (ANOVAs) (exposure; 4 levels, outcome; 2 levels, session; 2 levels) followed by Fischer’s test of least significant difference for pairwise post hoc comparisons. The main effect of sessions was used to ascertain whether a bias was present between data-sets obtained from session 1 vs. session 2.

Correlation analyses were conducted using the Pearson product-moment correlation coefficient between the reported itch areas at 5 or 10 min and the peak itch intensity/AUC and itch intensity at 5 min. Correlations were assessed within each itch provocation condition (i.e. n = 15) and across all 4 conditions for each session (i.e. n = 60).

Reliability statistics

To estimate the reliability between the 2 sessions the intra-class correlation coefficient (ICC) and the coefficient of variation (CV) were calculated. In addition, the estimated minimum sample sizes for parallel and cross-over studies were calculated and Bland-Altman plots were produced. To evaluate the relative reliability between the 2 sessions the ICC (model 2,1; randomly selected raters and single measurements) for absolute consistency was used. Further information is available on different ICC models elsewhere (41–43). The ICC was calculated for each of the 4 applied itch provocations for the parameters itch AUC and peak itch intensity. ICC values ranging from 0.5 to 0.75 were considered moderate, while values > 0.75 are considered excellent (44). In the calculations of CV and ICC values, subjects who rated zero in both sessions were excluded from reliability calculations pertaining to that particular condition (i.e. 3 subjects for the 5 spiculae condition and 1 subject for 15 spiculae condition). This was done to avoid artificially inflating the produced measures of intra-individual reliability in the lower stimulus intesity conditions, where zero was occasionally rated in both sessions. One significant outlier (subject number 6, 15 spiculae condition) was excluded from further reliability assessments. The data from subjects are included in all other analyses. CV was calculated both within- and between-sessions, signifying inter- and intra-individual measurements, respectively. The following equations were used (their respective theoretical frameworks are elaborated upon in (39, 45))

where SD represents the standard deviation and μ the mean for each session. For potential future use in clinical or exploratory studies minimally required sample size estimations were performed using the data obtained from each of the 4 itch provocations (5, 15, 25 spiculae, and 1% histamine). Using the following equations, the minimal amounts of participants in future parallel and cross-over studies were calculated. The power (1–b) was set to 0.8 and the significance level (a) at 0.05, using the standard textbook value (2×7.8=15.6) at the relevant α and β-levels (46, 47). The relevant detectable effect (E) was estimated as a 50% reduction of the mean itch intensity (itch AUC or peak itch intensity) averaged between sessions 1 and 2.

Bland-Altman-plots were produced to illustrate possible variations between the sessions, variations between the 4 itch provocations, outliers, and potential systematic biases. The difference, as calculated by subtraction of session 1 from session 2 for each parameter, was compared with the mean.

RESULTS

All of the 15 male subjects enrolled in this study completed both of the experimental sessions. The 2 sessions were conducted with 7.73 ± 0.83 days in between. No significant main effect of session was found pertaining to induced itch: F (1, 14 = 0.073, p = 0.791) or itch areas: F (1, 14=1.54, p = 0.235), indicating no significant bias between experimental sessions.

Itch intensity and pain sensations

No statistical differences in itch AUC or peak itch intensity were detected between the 2 sessions for neither histamine nor cowhage. Distinct 10 min temporal itch intensity profiles were recorded in response to the 4 itch provocations in each of the 2 sessions (Fig. 2). Peak itch intensity was achieved 2–4 min after both cowhage and histamine applications with the histamine-induced itch exhibiting a slower decline than cowhage. The 3 applications of cowhage (5, 15, and 25 spiculae) exhibited dose-responsiveness, both in terms of itch intensity AUC and itch peak intensity, with the 5 spiculae and the 25 spiculae applications being significantly different (p < 0.01 and p < 0.05, respectively; Fig. 2E). While a significant difference was found between the 5 spiculae condition and 1% histamine for the peak itch intensity (p < 0.05), the 1% histamine and the 25 spiculae, produced similar individual itch peak intensities at 4.09 ± 0.76 and 4.07 ± 0.77, respectively (p > 0.9) (Fig. 2F). The histamine resulted in significantly higher itch AUC values than any of the cowhage conditions (p < 0.05).


Fig. 2. Comparative figures of the temporal visual analogue scale (VAS) intensity for itch and “pricking/stinging” pain for each of the 4 itch provocations in session 1 and 2 (A: 1% histamine, B: 25 spiculae, C: 15 spiculae and D: 5 spiculae). VAS recordings of “burning” pain intensity are not included. (E) Mean of session 1 and 2 for AUC of itch intensity for each itch provocation. (F) Average of session 1 and 2 for VAS-peak of itch intensity for each itch provocation. Mean ± standard error of the mean (SEM), *p < 0.05, **p < 0.01.

The recorded pricking/stinging peak VAS intensities were in mean 1.07 ± 0.29 for all 3 cowhage applications and 1.44 ± 0.37 for histamine averaged between sessions. The temporal VAS profiles are shown in Fig. 2. Not all subjects experienced the itch provocations as pricking/stinging and the frequency of reports differed between conditions, e.g. in session 1 after application of 25 spiculae, 14/15 subjects reported some presence of pricking/stinging, while after application of 5 spicules only 6/15 subjects reported these sensations. For burning, the intensities reported were very low: 0.38 ± 0.26 for cowhage and 0.42 ± 0.24 for histamine (data not plotted), and only 6 of 15 subjects employed the “burning” quality during the course of the study. Lastly, several participants spontaneously reported that cowhage-elicited itch and pain sensations occurred in intense “on/off” bouts; however, this was unquantifiable in the present study design.

Spatial discrimination of cowhage- and histamine-induced itch

The size of the reported perceived areas of itch exhibited high intra- and inter-individual variation. No significant differences were found for the perceived areas of itch between the sessions for any of the itch provocations with or without pooling data from 5 and 10 min drawings and sessions. The area of perceived itch to cowhage was significantly larger at 5 min compared with 10 min after application (for pooled sessions and conditions) (p < 0.05), while no differences were observed for the size of the histamine-induced areas of itch at 5 min compared with 10 min after application for unpooled and pooled sessions (Fig. 3A). Moreover, no significant differences were found between the spatial distribution of cowhage- and histamine-evoked itch for any of the 4 applications including at similar peak intensities, i.e. between the 25 spiculae and the histamine applications (Fig. 4B).


Fig. 3. (A) Mean of session 1 and 2 of the total area of perceived itch (in % of total arm chart) at 5 and 10 min for 1% histamine and the 3 cowhage applications averaged. (B) Mean area of perceived itch (in % of total arm chart) pooled between sessions and time points (5 and 10 min) for each of the 4 itch provocations (5, 15, 25 spiculae and 1% histamine). Mean ± standard error of the mean (SEM), *p < 0.05.


Fig. 4. Overlays for the overall spatial distribution and localization of itch for each of the applied itch provocations. A: 5 spiculae, B: 15 spiculae, C: 25 spiculae, and D: 1% histamine. Sessions 1 and 2 are pooled.

Overlays of the spatial distributions of perceived itch are presented for 5, 15, 25 spiculae, and 1% histamine, with the 2 sessions being pooled (Fig. 4). As is apparent from Fig. 4A, the perception of low-intensity itch (5 spiculae) was difficult for the subjects to localize. No significant correlations were found between the perceived areas of itch and the rated itch intensity (AUC, peak or intensity at 5 min) within the individual provocations. Analyses across all 4 conditions in each of the 2 sessions yielded significant positive correlations between area size and itch intensity at 5 min in session 1: ρ = 0.27, p < 0.05, n = 60, and in session 2: ρ = 0.42, p < 0.01, n = 60.

Reliability of the applied models of itch

Table I shows the values of within-sessions CV, between-sessions CV, ICC values between sessions and estimated minimum sample sizes for the induced itch, measured as itch AUC and peak itch intensity. The inter-individual variation is generally high for induced itch, whereas the intra-individual variation is relatively low. All tested itch provocations exhibited moderate-to-excellent reliability estimated by ICC calculation. For the 5, 15 and 25 spiculae applications ICC-values ranged from 0.63 to 0.77 for itch AUC, and 0.57 to 0.72 for peak itch intensity, without coherence in terms of the influence of the number of spiculae inserted. However, the itch induced by 1% histamine yielded better ICC values than for the cowhage applications, i.e. 0.83 for itch AUC and 0.93 for peak itch intensity. Parameter comparison between itch AUC and peak itch intensity from 10 min recordings showed a better performance for achieving low and intra-individual variability by measuring individual peak itch intensity over itch AUC.


Table I. Overview of reliability metrics for cowhage- and histamine-induced itch

The within-sessions CV values showed stimulus-intensity-dependent improvements in absolute reliability, i.e. lower CV values for itch provocations associated with more intense itch (such as 25 spiculae or 1% histamine). A similar trend was observed for between-sessions CV values, indicating that increasing the stimulation intensity can reduce both intra- and inter-individual variability (Table I). For absolute reliability measured by CV within both sessions, the peak itch intensity parameter was superior to the itch AUC, with a CV across all itch provocations, which was, in mean, 22.24 ± 3.77% lower. This resulted in proportionally lower minimum required sample sizes for cross-over studies compared with parallel designs.

The Bland-Altman difference plots (Fig. 5) illustrate the level of agreement between the 2 sessions for itch AUC and peak itch intensity. The difference plots showed no evidence of a fixed or proportional bias. The Bland-Altman plots for peak itch intensity showed a smaller mean difference than for itch AUC for all itch provocations, indicating a better between-session consistency for peak itch intensity, especially between 25 spiculae and 1% histamine (Fig. 5).


Fig. 5. Bland-Altman difference plots. (A) itch visual analogue scale area under the curve (VAS AUC) and (B) itch VAS peak of itch intensity for each of the applied itch provocations (5, 15, 25 spiculae and 1% histamine), containing limits of agreement (meandifference± 1.96 SDdifference). Solid line represents mean difference between sessions and the dashed lines the limits of agreement.

DISCUSSION

The present study showed that insertion of 5, 15, or 25 cowhage spiculae and intra-epidermal delivered 1% histamine resulted in moderate-to-excellent relative reliability and moderate absolute reliability of the evoked itch responses. The overall reliability was best for the provocations causing higher itch intensities. A total of 25 spiculae evoked itch with a peak itch intensity comparable to that of 1% histamine. No differences were detected in the sizes of the itch areas reported for any of the itch provocations. The study provides the basis for calculating sample sizes for parallel or cross-over study designs employing human models of itch. Based on the reliability assessed here, cross-over designs are recommended whenever possible.

Sensory responses to histamine and cowhage

The temporal itch intensity profiles, evoked in response to the 4 itch models, are well aligned with previous research in terms of peak itch intensity and time course (8, 11, 13, 31). The peak itch intensity was generally reached within 2–4 min after administration and cowhage-induced itch exhibited a more rapid decrease than that of histamine, also in accordance with some of the previous findings (8, 48, 49). However, it should be noted that some previous studies have utilized a method of rubbing 40–45 spicules onto a larger skin area (11, 48, 50), and that, in some of these studies, this method is reported to produce a more intense itch (peak VAS ≈ 6–7) with less latency, which is probably associated with the application technique (11, 50). These discrepancies in the literature regarding the magnitude and quality of cowhage-evoked psychophysical responses highlight the problem of comparability between studies utilizing the cowhage model and standardizability of the model itself. Importantly, the model is reliant on diverse, untested plant material sourced from very different conditions across the world (9).

The 3 cowhage itch provocations caused stimulus-intensity dependent responses measured by itch AUC and peak itch intensity, as previously suggested (51, 52), whilst 25 spiculae and 1% histamine elicited very similar peak itch intensity. The SPT-delivered 1% histamine gave rise to a much longer-lasting itch than any of the cowhage applications, also in accordance with previous observations (11, 31).

These characteristic time/intensity profiles of itch induced by histamine and cowhage closely mimic the temporal profile of discharge rates recorded from histamine (53) or cowhage-responsive spinothalamic tract (STT) units in monkeys when their receptive fields were probed with histamine or cowhage, respectively (54).

Previous studies on cowhage- and histamine-induced itch have reported that the sensation of itch frequently occurs together with pricking/stinging and burning pain (3, 13, 51). The present study also showed pricking/stinging pain after both cowhage and histamine application, although not to the same extent as some previous publications (13, 33). In addition, very low levels of burning pain were observed and generally reported to be slightly lower than in previous studies (3, 13, 51). In the present study, 9/15 subjects never reported burning pain in response of any of the itch provocations across both sessions. These discrepancies may be caused by methodological differences relating to delivery method of histamine and cowhage, applied pain quantification scale, sampling frequency, and participants (who were exclusively male in the present study (55, 56)). Cowhage spiculae and, to a lesser extent, histamine have previously been described as eliciting itch occurring in “on/off” burst sensations (57), which was also reported by several subjects in this study. Notably, this on/off psychophysical scoring of itch sensation appears to correspond to observations made on primary afferents using microneurography, where cowhage spiculae in particular provoke a characteristic bursting pattern, with discharge frequencies during the burst of ≈0.5–5 Hz and silent intervals between bursts of ≈10 s (15, 58).

Assessment of the cowhage- and histamine-induced itch areas

Since PmC/Aδ-fibres (conveying cowhage-induced itch) have much smaller receptive fields and are more densely distributed than CMi-fibres (conveying histaminergic itch) (24, 25, 49), we hypothesized that itch induced by cowhage would permit higher spatial discriminatory resolution than itch induced by histamine. A previous study suggested that histaminergic itch is less localizable and expands over a larger area than cowhage-induced itch, in line with the theoretical spatial resolution of CMi- and Aδ/PmC-fibres. However, this observation was not quantified (7). In the present study, the analysis did not reveal a significantly larger area of itch perception for histamine than for cowhage, as previously suggested. It is unclear why cowhage-induced itch, which is in part Aδ-fibre driven, does not result in a more spatially confined and localizable itch area than that of histamine, which is conveyed via CMi-fibres, as this has been shown for Aδ- pricking and C-fibre burning pain (19–23). A potential confounding factor could be the initial mechanical stimulation associated with the application of cowhage and histamine and, in lieu of the high variability of the reported itch areas, it could be that the present study did not have adequate statistical power to detect such a difference. In addition, the bursting nature of the cowhage-induced itch sensation may have further decreased the reliability of the assessment, and it should be noted that the itch areas were not quantified during peak itch intensity, but always at 5 and 10 min after the itch provocations. In particular, for the cowhage-induced itch, which typically wears off more quickly than histamine-induced itch, this resulted in several area drawings being produced when the concurrent itch sensation was low. However, a non-quantified tendency towards more anatomically accurate localization of the areas of itch was observed for provocations eliciting more intense itch, i.e. 25 spiculae or 1% histamine, as opposed to 5 spiculae, despite the fact that these areas were not significantly smaller. While in the present study, the subjects did not observe the application of cowhage spicules or the SPT lancet introduction of histamine, both procedures are unavoidably associated with brief tactile/painful stimulation of the insertion area. Repeated vs. single insertion of spiculae, i.e. in the 5 vs. 25 spiculae conditions, may have produced the tendency towards more accurate localization of the areas of itch produced by 25 spiculae on the basis of more tactile noise rather than itch sensation even though the area drawings were recorded 5 min after itch provocations.

Sensory spatial characteristics and acuity of experimentally evoked itch and chronic itch occurring in patients (59) is a sparsely studied subject and it is methodologically difficult to assess without introducing biasing tactile stimuli. A study by Wahlgren & Ekblom (60) explored the 2-point discrimination of itch stimuli using histaminergic itch provocations and found exceedingly poor spatial acuity compared with what is established for tactile and, more recently, also for pain stimuli (60, 61).

Reliability of surrogate models of itch

Test–retest reliability studies have previously been performed for different human surrogate models of pain and hyperalgesia, e.g. the ultraviolet B (UVB)-model of cutaneous hyperalgesia, the capsaicin model of secondary hyperalgesia or the L-menthol model of cold allodynia (29, 39, 62–64). Previously, the methods for induction of histaminergic and non-histaminergic itch have been based on a variety of different approaches to application (8, 15, 25), and have not been assessed for test–retest reliability. The lack of knowledge on test–retest performance decreases: (i) accurate power/sample-size calculation, (ii) usefulness of surrogate models of itch for antipruritic drug candidate screening, and (iii) comparability between studies applying models of itch.

In line with the present study, human surrogate models of itch, in general, exhibit high inter-individual variability (9, 13, 65). Moreover, in accordance with previous studies (33, 51), it was found that larger amounts of cowhage spiculae inserted into the skin resulted in more intense itch. Not previously explored, the present study revealed that itch induced with either cowhage or histamine was characterized by high test–retest reliability within individual subjects and that itch provocations evoking higher itch peak intensities were characterized by better test–retest reliability. It is plausible that application of even more spiculae, e.g. 40–45 or even 70–100 as used in some previous studies, would further improve reliability outcomes (5, 11, 48, 50). Along these lines, administration of a range of concentrations of histamine, e.g. by the use of intra-dermal injections, would have clarified whether histamine-induced itch also exhibits better test–retest reliability when more vigorous responses are evoked.

The high variability in itch intensity ratings between subjects is conceivably attributable to the following factors: (i) central or peripheral individual differences in the neurophysiological processing of itch, i.e. a result of different physiological susceptibility to itch induced by the utilized substances (33, 51); and (ii) methodological variations in relation to the amount of inserted spiculae, variations in skin thickness (resulting in different penetration depth of the spiculae) or personality traits (9, 51). As with all psychophysical studies relying on self-reporting of sensory experiences, variation could, in part, be a result of diverse interpretations of the VAS, thereby ensuing in dissimilar ratings of itch between subjects resulting in large inter-subject, but not intra-subject, variation. Although the amount of spiculae applied to the skin was tightly controlled by using a digital micro-dermascope, it was not possible to accurately assess the penetration depth and angle in the epidermis. Previous studies have asserted that cowhage spiculae should be inserted near the basal membrane of the epidermis or at least into the stratum granulosum in order to evoke a sensory response (66). In addition, it has been suggested that in order to elicited a neuronal excitation of pruriceptive fibres the spiculae must be inserted adjacent to a nerve terminal (8, 57), which is less likely to occur when applying very few spiculae (33). On the other hand, a previous study found that insertion of a single cowhage spicule produced itch in 80% of all subjects (10), suggesting that the cowhage spiculae application is generally a robust approach. Lastly, 2 findings counter the notion of spiculae insertion variability as a major issue for model reliability: (i) a considerable effect should also be observable for measures of relative reliability measures, which is not the case; (ii) a detectable, albeit small, micro-flare reaction was generally observed post-application for all subjects, even those reporting no or very low itch in response to cowhage, indicating adequate insertion of physiologically active spiculae. Thus, individual neurophysiological or psychophysical differences should explain the majority of the observed variability. In the present study a highly homogenous group of young healthy male caucasian subjects was recruited to match typical conditions of phase 1 proof-of-concept studies in early drug development. It is highly conceivable that at least higher inter-individual disagreement would be present in a more diverse sample composition, i.e. on parameters such as gender, age, ethnicity and morbidity.

Another finding pertaining to the reliability assessment was that peak itch intensity generally performed better than itch AUC, both in terms of absolute and relative reliability. These are both very common outcome measures for quantifying itch in response to a pruritogenic stimulus and/or a potential intervention (11, 50, 65, 67, 68), and both measures can easily be assessed in most study designs. However, since peak itch intensity does not convey information about the temporal profile of the perceived itch (which is important in a clinical context), it may be less relevant as a primary outcome measure and could be exchanged for itch AUC. Moreover, based on the herein proposed sample size calculations adjusted by the obtained ICC values it would not drastically increase the minimum required sample size to use itch AUC instead of peak itch intensity in studies where a cross-over design is possible.

Conclusion

The present study is the first to assess and compare the test–retest reliability of human surrogate models of itch. Cowhage application using negative-grip tweezers and application of histamine with an SPT lancet and a pressure of 120 g showed moderate-to-excellent intra-individual test–retest reliability and moderate inter-individual reliability. Itch provocation with 1% histamine and, to a lesser extent, insertion of 25 spiculae proved to have the best intra-individual test–retest reliability in a 1-week observation period. Moderate inter-subject reliability and moderate-to-excellent intra-subject reliability favour cross-over study designs. The extent of the perceived area of itch did not differ between cowhage- and histamine-induced itch and, generally, the variability for this parameter was high.

ACKNOWLEDGEMENTS

The authors thank the volunteers for participating in this study and Frank Birkhøy Schultz Andersen for technical assistance and development of the computerized VAS. Cowhage spicules were kindly supplied by Professor Robert LaMotte, Yale University.

The authors have no conflicts of interest to declare.

REFERENCES
  1. Handwerker HO. Chapter 1 – itch hypotheses. In: Itch mechanisms and treatment, 1st edition. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press/Taylor & Francis, 2014.
    View article    Google Scholar
  2. Bautista DM, Wilson SR, Hoon MA. Why we scratch an itch: the molecules, cells and circuits of itch. Nat Neurosci 2014; 17: 175–182.
    View article    Google Scholar
  3. Dhand A, Aminoff MJ. The neurology of itch. Brain 2013; 137: 313–322.
    View article    Google Scholar
  4. Nakagawa H, Hiura A. Four possible itching pathways related to the TRPV1 channel, histamine, PAR-2 and serotonin. Malays J Med Sci 2013; 20: 5–12.
    View article    Google Scholar
  5. Gibson R a., Robertson J, Mistry H, McCallum S, Fernando D, Wyres M, et al. A randomised trial evaluating the effects of the TRPV1 antagonist SB705498 on pruritus induced by histamine, and cowhage challenge in healthy volunteers. PLoS One 2014; 9: e100610.
    View article    Google Scholar
  6. Schmelz M. Chemical response pattern of different classes of C-nociceptors to pruritogens and algogens. J Neurophysiol 2003; 89: 2441–2448.
    View article    Google Scholar
  7. Namer B, Carr R, Johanek LM, Schmelz M, Handwerker HO, Ringkamp M. Separate peripheral pathways for pruritus in man. J Neurophysiol 2008; 100: 2062–2069.
    View article    Google Scholar
  8. Johanek LM, Meyer R a, Hartke T, Hobelmann JG, Maine DN, LaMotte RH, et al. Psychophysical and physiological evidence for parallel afferent pathways mediating the sensation of itch. J Neurosci 2007; 27: 7490–7497.
    View article    Google Scholar
  9. Andersen HH, Elberling J, Arendt-Nielsen L. Human surrogate models of histaminergic and non-histaminergic itch. Acta Derm Venereol 2015; 95: 771–777.
    View article    Google Scholar
  10. Davidson S, Giesler GJ. The multiple pathways for itch and their interactions with pain. Trends Neurosci 2010; 33: 550–558.
    View article    Google Scholar
  11. Papoiu ADP, Tey HL, Coghill RC, Wang H, Yosipovitch G. Cowhage-induced itch as an experimental model for pruritus. A comparative study with histamine-induced itch. PLoS One 2011; 6: e17786.
    View article    Google Scholar
  12. Jeffry J, Kim S, Chen Z-F. Itch signaling in the nervous system. Physiology 2011; 26: 286–292.
    View article    Google Scholar
  13. Hartmann EM, Handwerker HO, Forster C. Gender differences in itch and pain-related sensations provoked by histamine, cowhage and capsaicin. Acta Derm Venereol 2015; 95: 25–30.
    View article    Google Scholar
  14. Schmidt R, Schmelz M, Weidner C, Handwerker HO, Torebjörk HE. Innervation territories of mechano-insensitive C nociceptors in human skin. J Neurophysiol 2002; 88: 1859–1866.
    View article    Google Scholar
  15. Johanek LM, Meyer RA, Friedman RM, Greenquist KW, Shim B, Borzan J, et al. A Role for polymodal C-fiber afferents in nonhistaminergic itch. J Neurosci 2008; 28: 7659–7669.
    View article    Google Scholar
  16. Reddy VB, Lerner EA. Plant cysteine proteases that evoke itch activate protease-activated receptors. Br J Dermatol 2010; 163: 532–5.
    View article    Google Scholar
  17. Lee SE, Jeong SK, Lee SH. Protease and protease-activated receptor-2 signaling in the pathogenesis of atopic dermatitis. Yonsei Med J 2010; 51: 808–822.
    View article    Google Scholar
  18. Højland CR, Andersen HH, Poulsen JN, Arendt-nielsen L, Gazerani P. A human surrogate model of itch utilizing the TRPA1 agonist trans-cinnamaldehyde. Acta Derm Venereol 2015; 95: 798–803.
    View article    Google Scholar
  19. Beissner F, Brandau A, Henke C, Felden L, Baumgärtner U, Treede R-D, et al. Quick discrimination of A(delta) and C fiber mediated pain based on three verbal descriptors. PLoS One 2010; 5: e12944.
    View article    Google Scholar
  20. Koltzenburg M, Handwerker HO, Torebjork HE. The ability of humans to localise noxious stimuli. Neurosci Lett 1993; 150: 219–222.
    View article    Google Scholar
  21. Schlereth T, Magerl W, Treede RD. Spatial discrimination thresholds for pain and touch in human hairy skin. Pain 2001; 92: 187–194.
    View article    Google Scholar
  22. Adriaensen H, Gybels J, Handwerker HO, Van Hees J. Response properties of thin myelinated (A-delta) fibers in human skin nerves. J Neurophysiol 1983; 49: 111–122.
    View article    Google Scholar
  23. Nahra H, Plaghki L. The effects of A-fiber pressure block on perception and neurophysiological correlates of brief non-painful and painful CO2 laser stimuli in humans. Eur J Pain 2003; 7: 189–199.
    View article    Google Scholar
  24. LaMotte RH, Dong X, Ringkamp M. Sensory neurons and circuits mediating itch. Nat Rev Neurosci 2014; 15: 19–31.
    View article    Google Scholar
  25. Ringkamp M, Schepers RJ, Shimada SG, Johanek LM, Hartke T V, Borzan J, et al. A role for nociceptive, myelinated nerve fibers in itch sensation. J Neurosci 2011; 31: 14841–14849.
    View article    Google Scholar
  26. Wilson S, Bautista D. Itching for relief. Nat Neurosci 2013; 16: 775–777.
    View article    Google Scholar
  27. van Laarhoven AIM, Kraaimaat FW, Wilder-Smith OH, van Riel PLCM, van de Kerkhof PCM, Evers AWM. Sensitivity to itch and pain in patients with psoriasis and rheumatoid arthritis. Exp Dermatol 2013; 22: 530–534.
    View article    Google Scholar
  28. Arendt-Nielsen L, Curatolo M, Drewes A. Human experimental pain models in drug development: translational pain research. Curr Opin Investig Drugs 2007; 8: 41–53.
    View article    Google Scholar
  29. Andersen HH, Olsen RV, Møller HG, Eskelund PW, Gazerani P, Arendt-Nielsen L. A review of topical high-concentration L-menthol as a translational model of cold allodynia and hyperalgesia. Eur J Pain 2013; 18: 315–325.
    View article    Google Scholar
  30. Klein T, Magerl W, Rolke R, Treede R-D. Human surrogate models of neuropathic pain. Pain 2005; 115: 227–233.
    View article    Google Scholar
  31. Papoiu ADP, Coghill RC, Kraft RA, Wang H, Yosipovitch G. A tale of two itches. Common features and notable differences in brain activation evoked by cowhage and histamine induced itch. Neuroimage 2012; 59: 3611–3623.
    View article    Google Scholar
  32. World Medical A. World Medical Association Declaration of Helsinki: ethical principles for medical research involving human subjects. JAMA 2013; 310: 2191–2194.
    View article    Google Scholar
  33. LaMotte RH, Shimada SG, Green BG, Zelterman D. Pruritic and nociceptive sensations and dysesthesias from a spicule of cowhage. J Neurophysiol 2009; 101: 1430–1443.
    View article    Google Scholar
  34. Andersen HH, Sørensen A-KR, Nielsen GAR, Mølgaard MS, Stilling P, Elberling J, et al. A reliability assessment of standardized human surrogate models of histaminergic and non-histaminergic itch using histamine and cowhage spicules. Acta Derm Venereol 2015; 95: 906.
    View article    Google Scholar
  35. Andersen HH, Lundgaard AC, Petersen AS, Hauberg LE, Sharma N, Hansen SD, et al. The lancet weight determines wheal diameter in response to skin prick testing with histamine. PLoS One 2016; 11: e0156211.
    View article    Google Scholar
  36. Woodward D, Conway J, Wheeler L. Cutaneous itching models. In Maibach H, Lowe N, editors. Models in dermatology, 1st edition. Basel: Krager, 1985: pp 187–195.
    View article    Google Scholar
  37. Darsow U, Ring J, Scharein E, Bromm B. Correlations between histamine-induced wheal, flare and itch. Arch Dermatol Res 1996; 288: 436–441.
    View article    Google Scholar
  38. Masse MS, Granger Vallée A, Chiriac A, Dhivert-Donnadieu H, Bousquet-Rouanet L, Bousquet PJ, et al. Comparison of five techniques of skin prick tests used routinely in Europe. Allergy Eur J Allergy Clin Immunol 2011; 66: 1415–1419.
    View article    Google Scholar
  39. Mørch CD, Gazerani P, Nielsen TA, Arendt-Nielsen L. The UVB cutaneous inflammatory pain model: a reproducibility study in healthy volunteers. Int J Physiol Pathophysiol Pharmacol 2013; 5: 203–215.
    View article    Google Scholar
  40. Boudreau SA, Badsberg S, Christensen SW, Egsgaard LL. Digital pain drawings: assessing touch-screen technology and 3D body schemas. Clin J Pain 2016; 32: 139–145.
    View article    Google Scholar
  41. Shrout PE, Fleiss JL. Intraclass correlations: uses in assessing rater reliability. Psychol Bull 1979; 86: 420–428.
    View article    Google Scholar
  42. Weir JP. Quantifying test-retest reliability using the intraclass correlation coefficient and the SEM. J Strength Cond Res 2005; 19: 231–240.
    View article    Google Scholar
  43. Yen M, Lo L-H. Examining test-retest reliability: an intra-class correlation approach. Nurs Res 2011; 51: 59–62.
    View article    Google Scholar
  44. Fleis J. The design and analysis of clinical experiments. 1st edition. New York: Wiley, 1986.
    View article    Google Scholar
  45. Lexell JE, Downham DY. How to assess the reliability of measurements in rehabilitation. Am J Phys Med Rehabil 2005; 84: 719–723.
    View article    Google Scholar
  46. Julious SA. Tutorial in biostatistics: sample sizes for clinical trials with Normal data. Stat Med 2004; 23: 1921–1986.
    View article    Google Scholar
  47. Bowers D. Medical statistics from scratch: an introduction for health professionals. 2nd edition. New York: Wiley, 2007.
    View article    Google Scholar
  48. Hawro T, Fluhr JW, Mengeaud V, Redoulés D, Church MK, Maurer M, et al. Polidocanol inhibits cowhage – but not histamine-induced itch in humans. Exp Dermatol 2014; 23: 922–923.
    View article    Google Scholar
  49. Weinkauf B, Dusch M, van der Ham J, Benrath J, Ringkamp M, Schmelz M, et al. Mechano-sensitive nociceptors are required to detect heat pain thresholds and cowhage itch in human skin. Eur J Pain 2016; 20: 215–222.
    View article    Google Scholar
  50. Papoiu A, Valdes-Rodriguez R, Nattkemper L, Chan Y, Hahn G, Yosipovitch G. A novel topical formulation containing strontium chloride significantly reduces the intensity and duration of cowhage-induced itch. Acta Derm Venereol 2013; 93: 520–526.
    View article    Google Scholar
  51. Sikand P, Shimada SG, Green BG, LaMotte RH. Similar itch and nociceptive sensations evoked by punctate cutaneous application of capsaicin, histamine and cowhage. Pain 2009; 144: 66–75.
    View article    Google Scholar
  52. Sikand P, Shimada SG, Green BG, LaMotte RH. Sensory responses to injection and punctate application of capsaicin and histamine to the skin. Pain 2011; 152: 2485–2494.
    View article    Google Scholar
  53. Andrew D, Craig AD. Spinothalamic lamina I neurons selectively sensitive to histamine: a central neural pathway for itch. Nat Neurosci 2001; 4: 72–77.
    View article    Google Scholar
  54. Davidson S, Zhang X, Khasabov SG, Moser HR, Honda CN, Simone D, et al. Pruriceptive spinothalamic tract neurons: physiological properties and projection targets in the primate. J Neurophysiol 2012; 108: 1711–1723.
    View article    Google Scholar
  55. Gazerani P, Andersen OK, Arendt-Nielsen L. A human experimental capsaicin model for trigeminal sensitization. Gender-specific differences. Pain 2005; 118: 155–163.
    View article    Google Scholar
  56. Bartley EJ, Fillingim RB. Sex differences in pain: a brief review of clinical and experimental findings. Br J Anaesth 2013; 111: 52–58.
    View article    Google Scholar
  57. McNeil B, Dong X. Chapter 12 – mrgprs as itch receptors; in: Itch mechanisms and treatment, 1st edition. Boca Raton, FL; CRC Press/Taylor & Francis, 2014.
    View article    Google Scholar
  58. Handwerker HO. Microneurography of pruritus. Neurosci Lett 2010; 470: 193–196.
    View article    Google Scholar
  59. Andersen HH, Sand C, Elberling J. Considerable variability in the efficacy of 8% capsaicin topical patches in the treatment of chronic pruritus in 3 patients with notalgia paresthetica. Ann Dermatol 2016; 28: 86–89.
    View article    Google Scholar
  60. Wahlgren CF, Ekblom A. Two-point discrimination of itch in patients with atopic dermatitis and healthy subjects. Acta Derm Venereol 1996; 76: 48–51.
    View article    Google Scholar
  61. Mancini F, Bauleo A, Cole J, Lui F, Porro CA, Haggard P, et al. Whole-body mapping of spatial acuity for pain and touch. Ann Neurol 2014; 75: 917–924.
    View article    Google Scholar
  62. Olsen RV, Andersen HH, Møller HG, Eskelund PW, Arendt-Nielsen L. Somatosensory and vasomotor manifestations of individual and combined stimulation of TRPM8 and TRPA1 using topical L-menthol and trans-cinnamaldehyde in healthy volunteers. Eur J Pain 2014; 18: 1333–1342.
    View article    Google Scholar
  63. Mahn F, Hüllemann P, Wasner G, Baron R, Binder A. Topical high-concentration menthol: reproducibility of a human surrogate pain model. Eur J Pain 2014; 18: 1248–1258.
    View article    Google Scholar
  64. Andersen HH, Poulsen J, Uchida Y, Nikbakht A, Arendt-Nielsen L, Gazerani P. Cold and L-menthol-induced sensitization in healthy volunteers – a cold hypersensitivity analogue to the heat/capsaicin model. Pain 2015; 156: 880–889.
    View article    Google Scholar
  65. Gazerani P, Pedersen NS, Drewes AM, Arendt-Nielsen L. Botulinum toxin type A reduces histamine-induced itch and vasomotor responses in human skin. Br J Dermatol 2009; 161: 737–745.
    View article    Google Scholar
  66. Kempkes C, Buddenkotte J, Cevikbas F, Buhl T SM. Chapter 11 – role of PAR-2 in neuroimmune communication and itch. In: Itch: mechanisms and treatment. Boca Raton, FL: CRC Press, Taylor & Francis Group, 2014.
    View article    Google Scholar
  67. Bromma B, Scharein E, Darsow U, Ring J. Effects of menthol and cold on histamine-induced itch and skin reactions in man. Neurosci Lett 1995; 187: 157–160.
    View article    Google Scholar
  68. Yosipovitch G, Szolar C, Hui XY, Maibach H. Effect of topically applied menthol on thermal, pain and itch sensations and biophysical properties of the skin. Arch Dermatol Res 1996; 288: 245–248.
    View article    Google Scholar